Module 5 Flashcards
baby biographies
detailed, systematic observations of individual children. But no one had studied children’s thinking with specific tasks or theorized about how and why children’s thinking changed over time. Piaget is considered the founder of the field of cognitive development and one of the most influential psychologists of all time.
assumption of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development
One of the key assumptions of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development is that children are active and motivated learners, rather than passive absorbers of information.
He viewed children as little scientists who constantly made and tested hypotheses about how the world works.
He also insisted that their knowledge and understanding is best formed though self discovery rather than explicit teaching.
For example, he once said,
“Each time one prematurely teaches a child something he could have discovered himself, that child is kept from inventing it and consequently from understanding it completely.”
This idea is known as constructivism
constructivism
the idea that humans construct their own knowledge through direct experience, as opposed to being taught concepts in the abstract.
Remember that constructivism is its own set of theories that contrast with nativism and empiricism.
You might also recognize that this idea drives some approaches to education, like when students learn through problem sets, experimentation, and discussion rather than only through direct instruction.
schemes
Piaget suggested that as children learn about the world, they form schemes, or mental structures.
One type of scheme is sensorimotor, meaning that it’s a tangible, physical concept, like an action.
In this example, babies might have a banging scheme where they bang an object on a table, or a sucking scheme where they suck on a soother, finger, or toy.
The second type of scheme is more abstract, because it’s a cognitive concept.
That can be something like the concept of what a dog is or what the word “tradition” means.
assimilation
assimilation, children incorporate new information into their existing schemes.
Another way of saying that is that they integrate or assimilate reality into their existing views.
The new information fits what they already know, so they’ve only just expanded their set of examples that involve that scheme.
Accommodation
Accommodation happens when the new information doesn’t fit the existing scheme, so the scheme must change a bit, or accommodate.
Another way of saying this is that the child has to change their views or thinking to better match reality.
Equilibration
Piaget suggested that assimilation and accommodation are usually in a state of balance, or equilibrium, with the child engaging in each in equal proportions.
But every so often, the child finds that their existing schemes aren’t cutting it: the new information doesn’t fit, and so they are constantly having to accommodate their schemes, rather than being able to assimilate new information.
This means that the balance is out of whack and that their current way of thinking doesn’t work.
In order to get back into a state of equilibrium, the child must drastically change the way they think in a process called equilibration.
Piaget thought that this process happened three times during development, prompting children to move from one developmental stage to the next.
In each new stage, children were proposed to think in an entirely different way from the previous stage.
discontinuous stages and domain general thinking
Piaget’s theory is a stage theory that proposes that development is discontinuous rather than gradual and continuous
Piaget also thought that each way of thinking exhibited a particular theme that cut across different areas of cognitive development, meaning that he thought that these themes were domain- general rather than specific to certain domains like language or understanding of numbers.
Piaget thought that these stages were universal, occurring in children of all cultures across all parts of the world.
The sensorimotor stage
Piaget thought that children’s thinking was limited to their physical experiences in the world.
The other stages all have the word operations in them, which is the term Piaget used to refer to reversible mental actions, or the ability to perform actions on objects or on the world in your own mind.
Performing mental operations means that you can imagine things without them having to happen and perform “what if” scenarios in your head.
Birth - 2 years
lacked object permanence, the understanding that objects continue to exist when hidden.
Piaget proposed that the sensorimotor stage can be broken down into 6 substages. At first, babies only have automatic reflexes to work with.
By accidentally producing interesting outcomes, like grabbing something and then dropping it, or putting a new juicy toy in their mouth, babies start to learn how to reproduce those outcomes.
They also begin the process of gaining object permanence and being able to hold mental representations, or concepts, in their heads.
The sensorimotor stage
basic search task
is done by hiding an object under something like a blanket or pillow, and waiting to see if the baby searches for it. Let’s watch this baby go through it.
The sensorimotor stage
A-not-B error task
there are two hiding places instead of one.
The researcher starts by hiding a toy in location A several times and allowing the baby to retrieve it.
After a few repetitions, the researcher hides the toy in location B while the baby is watching. Babies younger than about 10 to 12 months tend to make the A-not-B error by searching in location
A, rather than location B. Piaget suggested that this is because babies do not yet have full object permanence. Instead, their representation of the object is tied to the action that they used to get it.
So in their minds, the object isn’t a concept that is distinct or separate from their previous reaching action, at least when it’s hidden.
The preoperational stage
2-7 years
they have mental representations and can engage in symbolic thought.
For example, children between 2 and 7 years of age can play pretend, they can use language, which is a system of abstract symbols, and they can hold a concept in memory for an extended period.
This means that their thinking is no longer limited to what they can experience in the physical world.
On the other hand, Piaget argued that their thinking was intuitive rather than logical, and that they couldn’t yet perform reversible mental actions in their mind, or operations.
The preoperational stage
Conservation tasks
test whether a child understands that certain properties like volume remain the same even after an object undergoes a transformation and now looks different.
The classic task is the conservation of liquids task. The child is shown two glasses of liquid, and the researcher confirms with the child that both glasses contain the same amount.
The researcher then pours the liquid from one glass into a taller, skinner glass in front of the child. If asked whether the two glasses have the same amount of liquid, or if one has more, a preoperational child will now say that the taller, skinner glass has more. It works with other properties too, like if you have two rows with the same number of coins and then spread one row out, the child will now think there are more coins in the spread-out row.
When asked why children think there is now more in the one case, they say things like “it’s tall” or “it’s long.”
Piaget suggested that preoperational children can’t yet perform operations, or mental actions on objects.
This means that their thinking is irreversible: they can’t imagine the liquid being poured back into the original-sized container, which would help them see that it’s the same. Instead, they exhibit centration, meaning that they focus on one aspect—in this case, the height of the liquid—while ignoring other aspects because they can’t keep both aspects in their heads at once.
The preoperational stage
three mountains task.
The child is placed in front of a display that has various objects placed around it.
From any one side, the child can see some of the objects, but not all of them.
The child is first asked to describe what they see from where they’re sitting.
Next, the child is moved to another side of the display and again asked to describe what they see, while the experimenter sits in View 1: the child’s original position.
When the child is asked to describe what the experimenter sees, even though they were just sitting there a minute ago, they still describe what they are currently seeing in View 2.
This is known as egocentrism, where children have difficulty putting themselves in another person’s shoes.
Instead, they’re entirely focused on their own views and experiences.
The concrete operations stage
7-11 years
Children in the concrete operations stage are less egocentric, and they can perform mental operations, passing the previous tasks. The reason the stage is called “concrete” though is because their ability to perform mental actions is limited to real-world, concrete situations. They have difficulty reasoning through abstract or hypothetical situations that don’t reflect the true state of the world. They also have difficulty with deductive reasoning, especially when it comes to abstract or hypothetical situations. This means that they have a hard time making specific conclusions from more general rules or information.
The concrete operations stage
Feather with glass task
You first start with a concrete, real-world situation.
You tell the child, “Suppose this rule is true: if you hit a glass with a hammer, it will break. Johnny hit a glass with a hammer, what happened?” The child will typically answer that the glass will break because hammers are hard.
But if you repeat the same thought experiment using an abstract, hypothetical situation that doesn’t exist in the real world, children in the concrete operations stage have a hard time.
For example, you would say, “Now suppose that this rule is true: if you hit a glass with a feather, it will break. Johnny hit a glass with a feather, what happened?”
The child is likely to say that the glass won’t break because feathers are soft.
This task shows that children have difficulty taking a hypothetical, general principle and applying it to a specific situation.
The concrete operations stage
pendulum task
shows that children in this stage also have problems with inductive reasoning, which is making general conclusions from data or observations.
In this task, children experiment to decide what makes a pendulum swing faster: is it the length of the string? the weight of the pendulum? the height at which the pendulum is released?
In order to solve this problem successfully, children will need to systematically test each variable by examining its effect while holding all of the others constant.
In other words, test whether the length of string makes a difference by trying different string lengths but keeping the weight and height the same, and so on.
Children will also need to systematically keep track of their findings as they go by recording them.
Children in the concrete operations stage tend to just combine different variables willy nilly, failing to attack the problem systematically and failing to write down anything they tried.
Not surprisingly, they don’t often come to the right conclusion, which is that only the length of string matters.
The formal operations stage
11+ years
think like adults. They can imagine hypothetical or abstract scenarios and reason through them, and they can engage in deductive and inductive reasoning.
This makes them better problem-solvers because they can imagine various scenarios without having to try them all.
Piagets contributions
Piaget was one of the main founders of the field of cognitive development, having a huge influence on our understanding of how children think and how we can help them learn.
Piaget demonstrated, for example, that children are not just little adults, but that, in fact, they think in very different ways.
Piaget also came up with a number of tasks and questions that probed children’s thinking and provided testable hypotheses that could be examined with more systematic research.
Piaget’s ideas about children’s learning also had a big impact on the field of education and are still evident in methods like discovery learning, experiential learning, or problem-based learning, where students learn through trying things rather than being told the answers.
What we have learned since Piaget
One issue was that Piaget based his observations on a small number of children from a certain cultural and sociodemographic background, including his own children.
This is a particular problem given that he claimed that the stages he proposed are universal, and later research found that there are some cultural differences in terms of the ages at which children pass his tasks.
Because Piaget was limited to his own observations—those infant behavioural methods we covered in Module 1 didn’t yet exist after all— he also tended to underestimate younger children and especially infants’ understanding.
On the other hand, Piaget also overestimated how well adolescents and adults can reason, and it turns out that they aren’t always completely logical and able to think hypothetically and abstractly.
Piaget didn’t really focus on differences between individual children, especially differences that might be due to the child’s social environment or culture
What we have learned since Piaget
horizontal décalage
Piaget also started to notice that development didn’t necessarily follow his strict stage-like proposal.
For example, he noticed that children tended to pass certain conservation tasks, like conservation of number, before others, like conservation of liquid.
He called this horizontal décalage, the idea that once children learn how to solve a particular problem, they don’t necessarily apply the same process or concept to other tasks.
But if children pass these tasks because they understand the concept of reversibility, or because they’re no longer centred on one aspect while ignoring others, then why don’t they pass all the tasks of the same kind at once?
Other researchers suggest that it’s because although there might be some stage-like patterns in development, it’s much more continuous than Piaget proposed.
This also means that contrary to what Piaget proposed, development is not completely domain-general.
If it were, then once children understood a certain concept like reversibility, then it should extend to tasks in different domains, like counting vs. understanding of volume.
Instead, development seems to include both domain-general and domain-specific aspects.
Lev Vygotsky’s assumptions
He believed that cognitive development was inherently social, shaped by interaction with other people and influenced by the cultural context in which a child grows up.
Cultures influence how valued different cognitive activities are, what tools are available to solve problems, and what practices are used to think, solve problems, and communicate with others.
Vygotsky argued that cognitive development was like an apprenticeship, where learning occurs best when a novice is paired with someone more knowledgeable and experienced, like their parents or older siblings.
In this way, the child is learning how to think like an adult of their culture, which is a very social activity.
Individualist cultures tend to emphasize independent learning and problem-solving, like in much of North America, but collectivist cultures are more likely to emphasize collaboration and group consensus.
zone of proximal development
the difference between what a child can do alone vs. what they can do with help.
The interesting space is what is in between those two points: what children can do with help.
Depending on how new they are to the task and how hard it is, they may need only a little bit of help, or they may need a lot.
Vygotsky thought that this was the prime zone for learning, and that the right amount of guidance could push children’s learning and potential to the highest point.
Scaffolding
Scaffolding refers to help or guidance that is appropriate for the learner’s needs: not too much, and not too little.
A brand-new learner might need more explicit instructions and help, whereas a more experienced learner needs the adult to back off a bit and provide hints or reminders instead, when needed.
A good teacher or parent will recognize each child’s zone of proximal development and be able to provide the right amount of scaffolding to encourage learning.