Midterm #2 Flashcards
Sensation
Refers to how cells of the nervous system detect stimuli in the environment (such as light, sound, heat, etc.), and how they transducer (convert) these signals into a change in membrane potential and neurotransmitter release.
Perception
Refers to the conscious experience and interpretation of sensory information
Sensory neurons
Specialized cells that detect a specific category of physical events, such as: the presence of specific molecules (smell, taste, nausea, pain), the presence or absence of physical pressure
(touch, stretch, vibration), the temperature (heat, cold, pain), the pH of a liquid (sour taste, suffocation, pain), electromagnetic radiation (vision).
Sensory Transduction
Sensory neurons have specialized receptors that transduce sensory stimuli into a change in membrane potential. Come in all shapes and sizes. Many sensory neurons do not have axons or action potentials, but they all release neurotransmitter. Sensory neurons that do not have action potentials release neurotransmitter in a graded fashion, dependent on their membrane potential. The more depolarized they are, the more neurotransmitter they release.
Photoreceptor cells
The sensory neurons responsible for vision. These cells transduce the electromagnetic energy of visible light into a change in membrane potential, which affects how much neurotransmitter they release. These cells do not have action potentials.
Opsins
Light-sensitive proteins. The opsins in photoreceptor cells are metabotropic receptors. They are only sensitive to light because they bind a molecule of retinal, which changes shape in response to light. The change in the shape of retinal is what activates this metabotropic receptor.
Retinal
Small molecule (synthesized from vitamin A) that attaches to the opsin proteins in the photoreceptor cells in our eyes. The retinal molecule is technically what absorbs the electromagnetic energy of visible light that allows us to see.
The two configurations of the retinal molecule
When retinal absorbs a wavelength of visible light, it activates the opsin protein (a metabotropic receptor). This launches an intracellular G protein signalling cascade that changes the membrane potential of the photoreceptor cell, affecting how much neurotransmitter it releases.
4 types of photoreceptor cells that contribute to vision and their metabotropic opsin protein
- Red cone cells express the red cone opsin.
- Green cone cells express the green cone opsin.
- Blue cone cells express the blue cone opsin
- Rod cells express the rhodopsin opsin. (The last to evolve; 100 times more sensitive to light than cone cells).
Cone Photoreceptors: Trichromatic Coding
Blue cone opsins are most sensitive to short wavelengths of light. Green cone opsins are most sensitive to medium wavelengths of light. Red cone opsins are most sensitive to long wavelengths of light.
Color perception is a function of the relative rates of activity across the three types of cone cells (i.e. colors are discriminated by the ration of activity across these cells.)
Trichromatic Coding
The key consideration for our brain for identifying color is how much each type of cone cell is activated relative to its maximum level. Each color of the rainbow corresponds to a particular pattern of activation across the three types of cones cells.
How is yellow light created
When red and green light bulbs are so close together that our eyes can’t differentiate them, the color looks yellow to us. Green light (530nm) activates the green cone opsin more than the red cone opsin. Red light (680nm) activates the red cone opsin more than the green cone opsin. The combination of red and green light causes the red and green cone opsin to be activated at similar amounts, which is what happens with yellow light (580nm).
Additive Color (Light)
Primary colours of light: Green, Red, Blue. Combine to make yellow, cyan, magenta. Sunlight is white light, since it contains an equal mixture of all the colors.
Our perception of light and colour has three dimensions to it
- Brightness - intensity (luminance, amount)
- Saturation - purity (in terms of wavelength mixture)
- Hue - dominant wavelength (colour)
Perceptual Dimensions of colour & light
If brightness is zero, the image is completely black. Hue and saturation have no impact without brightness. If there is (bright) light, the next question is whether the light is saturated with a particular wavelength (colour). If saturation is 0% then you are in the middle of the colour cone where there is an equal contribution from all visible wavelengths. An image with 0% saturation is grayscale (black and white), because all wavelengths are present in equal amounts. If saturation is >0%, the hue indicates the colour that the light is saturated with.
Protanopia
Absence of the red cone opsin (1% of males). People with this condition have trouble distinguishing colours in the green-yellow-red spectrum. Visual acuity is normal because red cone cells switch to using the green cone opsin. Simple mutations of the red cone opsin price less pronounced deficits in colour vision.
Deuteranopia
Absence of the green cone poison (1% of males). People with this condition have trouble distinguishing colours in the green-yellow-red spectrum. Visual acuity is normal because green cone cells switch to using the red cone opsin. Simple mutations of the green cone opsin (6% of males) produce less pronounced deficits in colour vision.
Tritanopia
Absence of the blue cone opsin (1% of the population). Blue cone cells do not compensate for this in any way, but since the blue cone opsin is no that sensitive to light anyway, visual acuity is not noticeably affected).
Visible light
Refers to electromagnetic energy that has a wavelength between 380 and 760 nm. We detect this light using four kinds of photoreceptor cells ( 1 rod & 4 cone cells).
The cornea
The outer, front layer of the eye. It focuses incoming light a fixed amount.
The conjunctiva
Is a mucous membranes that line the eyelid.
The sclera
Is opaque and does not permit entry of light.
The iris
A ring of muscle. The contraction and relaxation of this muscle determine the size of the pupil, which determines how much light will enter the eye.
The lens
Consists of several transparent layers. We change the shape of this lens to focus near versus far, a process known as accommodation.
Retina
The interior lining (furthest back part) of the eye is the retina. Photoreceptor cells are located in the furthest back layer of the retina.
Vitreous humor
Light passes through the lens and crosses the vitreous humor, a clear, gelatinous fluid.
Fovea
The central region of the retina is called the fovea. It primarily contains cone cells.
The periphery of the retina
Only contains rod cells
The optic disk
Where blood vessels enter and leave the eye. It is also where the optic nerve exits the eye, carrying visual information to the brain. There are no photoreceptors in this spot, so it is a blind spot.
Saccadic eye movements
Our eyes scan a scene by making saccadic eye movements - rapid, jerky shifts in gaze from one point to another.
Pursuit movements
When we maintain focus on an object that is moving (relative to us). This is the only time our eyes appear to calm down and move smoothly and slowly.
Orbits
Eyes are suspended in bony sockets in the front of the skull called orbits.
The sclera
The tough, outer white of the eye. Six extra ocular muscles are attached to the sclera. These muscles rotate the eye and hold it in place.
Organization of the retina
Visual inforamtion propagates from photoreceptor cells –> bipolar cells –> retinal ganglion cells –> brain. When light enters our eyes, it must pass through each of the cell layers in the retina before it can reach the opsin proteins in photoreceptor cells. There does not seem to be a good reason for this award arrangement.
Retina Fovea
In fovea, there is an equal number of photoreceptor cells, bipolar cells, and retinal ganglion cells. This means there is no compression of information. The fovea is the only part of our retina where our visual acuity is good enough to read text (20/20 vision). And the photoreceptor cells in our fovea are mostly cone cells, which support colour vision, so the fovea supports high resolution, colour vision. Fovea supports high resolution colour Vision but only when there is sufficient amount of light. At night, the moon must be at least half full for us to see in colour.
Periphery
Outside the fovea (in the periphery of retina), there is a massive compression (averaging) of information, since there are 100x more photoreceptor cells than retinal ganglion cells. Our visual acuity in peripheral vision is about 20/200, which is quite blurry. It is also grey scale. We can make out general shapes but not details. Yet, the periphery contains a high density of rod cells, which are sensitive to light, allowing us to easily. detect dim light and movements of light. While the peripheral vision is sensitive to dim light, it only provides low resolution grayscale images. What we see in peripheral vision 20 feet away is as detailed as what we see in our fovea 200 feet away.
Cones vs Rods
Cones: Most prevalent in the central retina; found in the fovea. Sensitive to moderate-to-high levels of light. Provide information about hue. Provide excellent acuity.
Rods: Most prevalent in the peripheral retina; not found in the fovea. Sensitive to low levels of light. Provide only monochromatic information. Provide poor acuity.
Neurons in the retina - photoreceptor cells
Located in the furthest back part of the retina. They express the opsin proteins that transducer light. Synapse on bipolar cells.
Neurons in the retina - bipolar cells
Relay information from photoreceptor cells to retinal ganglion cells.
Neurons in the retina - retinal ganglion cells
The only cells that send information out of the eye. Their axons form the optic nerve, which exits the retina through the optic disc (the blind spot of the retina).
Neurons in the retina - horizontal/amacrine cells
Horizontal cells and amacrine cells interconnect cells within each layer, which gives rise to complex interactions between neighbouring cells (within a layer).
Visual information pathways - visual ganglion cell action potentials
Retinal ganglion cells have action potentials, unlike most other cells in retina. Their axons go to 3 places:
1. Thalamus (specifically the lateral geniculate nucleus), which in turn projects to primary visual cortex (area V1) in the occipital lobe where visual information enters consciousness. This creates an internal (mental) representation of your entire visual space.
2. Midbrain (specifically the superior colliculi): Visual information is used here to control fast visually-guided reflexive movements. The midrabin doesn’t know what you are looking at, but it can draw attention to unexpected visual events.
3. Hypothalamus: Visual information is used here to control circadian rhythms such as sleep-wake cycles. The hypothalamus doesn’t know what you are looking at, but It knows how much light is present in your environment.
Predictive coding theory
Theory of sensory processing. The idea is that each node in the network tries to predict what its ascending inputs will look like in the next moment, based on previous experience. Top-down (descending) activity represents sensory predictions that neutralize any correctly predicted bottom-up ascending signals. Thus, what propagates up through the network may only be prediction signals, which inform the brain of how the current moment differs from what was expected. The prediction error signals that ascend through the network would cause learning to improve future precautions.
Receptive field
The receptive field of a neuron is a description of the (external) stimuli that activate it. For a neuron involved in visual processing, its receptive field is where light must be in visual space and what properties it must have to change the activity of the cell. It is in an area of visual space relative to a fixation point.
How to identify the receptive field of a cell involved in visual processing
We record the cell’s activity as the animal maintains focus on one spot on a computer screen (a fixation point). We then systematically shine light in different areas of the monitor to determine where in visual space the presence of light influences the activity of the cell. Once we find where the receptive field is, we determine if the cell responds differently to different colours or patters of light in that location.
Photoreceptor cell receptive fields
The first cell in the pathway. Photoreceptor cells release glutamate in a graded fashion dependent on their membrane potential: the more depolarized they are, the more glutamate they release. But their response to light are opposite of what you might expect. In complete darkness, photoreceptor cells sit at -40mV. This is their resting membrane potential. At rest, photoreceptor cells continuously release glutamate. When activated by light, photoreceptor cells hyper polarize to -70mV and stop releasing glutamate.
The Dark Current
Photoreceptor cells express an uncommon “leak” sodium ion channel that sits open at baseline (in the dark). The influx of sodium ions through these ion channels (the dark current) causes photoreceptor cells to sit at -40mV, where they continuously release glutamate. When an opsin protein absorbs light, it launches an intracellular g-protein signalling cascade that closes the open sodium ion channels. The closing of these ion channels causes the membrane to hyperpolarize to -70mV, at which point the photoreceptor cell stops releasing glutamate. All photoreceptor cells work like this. All the opsin proteins responsible for our conscious perception of vision are inhibitory metabotropic receptors.
Bipolar cells receptor fields
Second cell in the pathway. Bipolar cells do not have action potentials. Like photoreceptor cells, they release glutamate in a graded manner dependent on their membrane potential. There are two types of bipolar cells: OFF bipolar cells and ON bipolar cells.
OFF bipolar cells
Express normal excitatory ionotropic glutamate receptors, so their activity patterns follow that the photoreceptor cells that connect with them. In the dark, when photoreceptors are depolarized (-40mv) and releasing glutamate, OFF bipolar cells will also be depolarized and releasing glutamate. In the presence of light, when photoreceptors are hyperpolarized (-70mv) and not releasing glutamate, OFF bipolar cells will also by hyper polarized and not releasing glutamate.
ON bipolar cells
Have the opposite pattern of activity from OFF bipolar cells because they only express inhibitory (metabotropic) glutamate receptors. So, in the dark, when photoreceptor cells are releasing glutamate, ON bipolar cells will be hyperpolarized and not releasing glutamate. In the light, ON bipolar cells depolarize and release glutamate.
Retinal Ganglioon Cells (RGCs)
Are typical neurons. They have normal actions potentials and express normal excitatory ionotropic glutamate receptors.
Horizontal cells
Interconnect neighbouring photoreceptor cells. They regulate the amount of glutamate that is released from photoreceptor cells based on the activity of their neighbours. They compare the activity of neighbouring photoreceptor cells. They recognize that the centre photoreceptor cell is getting less light than its neighbours, and they accentuate this difference by counteracting the small light-induced hyperpolarization in the dimly lit cell. Thus, horizontal cells depolarize the “axon terminal” of photoreceptor cells according to how brightly lit the neighbouring photoreceptor cells are. Also release glutamate in a graded manner dependent on their membrane potential.
Bipolar cell receptive fields
The influence of horizontal cells creates a “centre-surround” organization in his receptive fields of bipolar cells.
Retinal Ganglion Cell Receptive Fields
Third cell in the pathway: Retinal ganglion cells have action potentials and a baseline firing rate in the dark. They inherit their receptive fields form bipolar cells, so they also have a “centre-surround” organization and are classified as ON or OFF cells.
ON retinal ganglion cells
Increase their rate of spiking when light is in the centre of their receptive field. They decrease their rate of spiking when light is brighter in the surround area of the receptive field.
OFF retinal ganglion cells
OFF retinal ganglion cells show the opposite pattern. They decrease their rate of spiking when light is in the centre of the receptive field and increase their rate of spiking when light is in the surround areas.
Retinal ganglion cell receptive fields in the fovea
Retinal ganglion cells in the fovea process colour information. They integrate information from many bipolar cells and have these types of receptive fields: Red on, green off. Green on, red off. Yellow on, blue off. Blue on, yellow off. (On = small circe in middle, Off = bigger surrounding circle).
Receptive fields in thalamus and V1
Retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) transmit visual information from the retina to the thalamus (the lateral geniculate nucleus). Thalamic neurons relay the information to primary visual cortex (area V1). The receptive fields of thalamic neurons are similar to that of the retinal ganglion cells. The receptive fields of neurons in V1 are the sum of many LGN neurons.
Simple cells in V1
Are sensitive to lines of light, and their receptive fields are typically organized in a center-surround fashion.
Receptive fields in primary visual cortex (V1 or striate cortex)
Neurons in V1 spike when there is a line of light in a particular orientation in their receptive field. The cell is most responsive to a vertical line of light in the centre of its receptive field. Some V1 neurons respond best to vertical lines, some to horizontal lines, and some to lines oriented somewhere in between.
Cortical columns in primary visual cortex
Every spot in your visual field is rigorously analyzed by a cortical column in V1. All neurons within a cortical column analyze the same area of visual space. Together, they analyze the orientation of light in the associated receptive field. The location of sharp transitions in the contrast/colour of light reveals borders, edges, and corners. Neurons downstream of V1 put all of this information together to identify objects and their position in space.
Visual association cortex
> 25% of the cerebral cortex is dedicated to processing visual information. All of the occipital lobe that is not primary visual cortex is considered visual association cortex. It extends to the parietal and temporal lobes, forming respectively the dorsal and ventral streams of visual info processing. Different areas of the visual association cortex are sensitive to different features of the visual environment.
Striate cortex
Part of visual association cortex. Is synonymous with primary visual cortex (Area V1).
Extrastriate cortex
Part of visual association cortex. Is synonymous with visual association cortex (areas V2, V3, V4, etc.)
Dorsal stream
The dorsal stream of visual information starts in primary visual cortex and ends in posterior parietal lobe. It is involved in identifying spatial location. It encodes where objects are, of they are moving, and how you should move to interact with them or avoid them.
The ventral stream
Starts in primary visual cortex and ends in inferior temporal lobe. It is involved in identifying form (shape). It encodes what the object is and its colour.
Monocular vision
Some V1 neurons respond to visual input from just one eye.
Binocular vision
Most V1 neurons respond to visual input from both eyes.
Depth perception
There are many monocular cues that can be used to estimate depth, such as relative size, amount of detail, relative movement as we move our eyes, etc. These are the cues we use to appreciate depth when looking at a photograph or TV screen (any flat, 2-dimensional image). Only the eye is required to perceive depth with monocular cues.
Stereopsis
The perception of depth that emerges from the fusion of two slightly different projections of an image on the two retinas. The difference between the images from the two eyes is called retinal disparity. It results from the horizontal separation of the two eyes. It improves the precision of depth perception, especially for moving objects. Two eyes are helpful when playing sports, but also (to some extent) when pouring a glass of water.
Agnosia
A deficit in the ability to recognize or comprehend certain sensory information, like specific features of objects, people, sounds, shapes, or smells, although the specific sense is not defective nor is there any significant memory loss. Relates to a problem in some sensory association cortex, not to problems that relate to the sensory neurons themselves or to the primary sensory areas.
Akinetopsia
A deficit in the ability to perceive movement - is a type of visual agnosia caused by damage to the dorsal visual stream in the parietal lobe of the cerebral cortex.
Cerebral achromatopsia
In contrast to regular chromatopsia, it is a visual agnosia caused by damage to the cerebral cortex in the ventral visual stream. People deny have any perception of colour. They say everything looks dull or drab, and that it is all just “shades of grey”. (People born with regular achromatopsia don’t say those things, because they have no conception of colour).
Prosopagnosia
Failure to recognize particular people by sight of their faces; a visual agnosia caused by damage to the fusiform gyrus (fusiform face area) in the ventral visual stream.
Sound waves
When an object vibrates, it causes molecules in the surrounding air to alternately condense and rarefy (pull apart). These fluctuations in air pressure give rise to a sound wave that travels away from the object at approximately 700 miles per hour.
Audition (hearing)
The human ear can transducer fluctuations in air pressure when the length of the sound wave is between 0.017 and 17 meters long. These sound waves are generated when physical objects vibrate between 20 and 20,000 times per second.
Three physical dimensions of sound
- Loudness
- Pitch (tone)
- Timbre
Loudness
Corresponds to the amplitude or intensity of the molecular vibrations, the relative difference in the density of air molecules between compressed and rarified air. This dimensions determines how far the sound wave will travel.
Pitch (tone)
Corresponds to the frequency of the molecular vibrations (or the distance between neighbouring peaks of compressed air). It is measured in hertz (Hz, cycles per second). Every frequency has a corresponding wavelength.
Timbre
Corresponds to the complexity of the sound wave. Our brains learn to recognize the timbre of sound waves to identify the source of the sound (e.g., which instruments is playing the note middle C).
Pinna
Sound is funnelled through the pinna (outer ear).
Tympanic membrane
At the end of the ear canal (still outer ear), sounds cause the tympanic membrane (the eardrum) to vibrate. These vibrations are transferred to the middle ear.
Ossicles
The middle ear is comprised of three ossicles (small bones): the malleus, incus and stapes.
Oval window
Vibrations of the tympanic membrane cause the ossicles to vibrate, which in turn cause the membrane behind the oval window to vibrate.
Cochlea
Vibrations of the oval window are transmitted to the fluid-filled cochlea (the inner ear), which is a long coiled tube-like structure that contains sensory neurons.
Basilar Membrane
High pitched notes are detected where the basilar membrane is thick and narrow (closest to the oval window). Low notes are detected where the basilar membrane is thin and wide. The basilar membrane is kind of like a xylophone backwards, where the longest wood bars correspond to the low notes.
The cochlea is divided into three longtudinal divisions
Scala vestibuli, scala media, and scala tympani.
Organ of Corti
The receptive organ. It consists of the basilar membrane on the bottom, the tectorial membrane on the top, and auditory hair cells in the middle.
Hair cells and Cilia
The cells that transduce sound are called hair cells because of their physical appearance. Their hair-like extensions are called cilia. Outer hair cells have cilia that are physically attached to the rigid tectorial membrane. The cilia of inner hair cells are not attached to anything. They sway back and forth with the movement of the solution. Sound waves cause the basilar membrane to move relative to the bacterial membrane, which causes hair cell cilia to stretch and bend. The movement of the cilia pulls open ion channels, which changes the membrane potential of hair cells.
Inner & Outer Hair Cells
Although there are 3 times more outer hair cells than inner hair cells, only inner hair cells transmit auditory information to the brain. Outer hair cells act like muscles to adjust the sensitivity of the tectorial membrane to vibrations. By regulating the flexibility of the tectorial membrane, outer hair cells influence the sensitivity of inner hair cells to specific frequencies of sound (i.e., different notes). People who do not have working inner hair cells are completely deaf. People who do not have functional outer hair cells can hear, but not very well.
Tip links
The cilia of hair cells are connected to each other by tip links - elastic filaments that attach the tip of one cilium to the side of adjacent cilium.
Insertional plaque
The point of attachment of a top link to cilium is called an insertional plaque. Each insertional plaque has a single ion channel in it that opens and closes according to the amount of stretch exerted by the tip link.
Loud Noise and tip link breakage
Lord noises can easily break the tip links that interconnect each cilia. And hair cells cannot transmit auditory information without tip links. Fortunately, tip links usually grow back within a few hours. Tip link breakage generally corresponds to temporary hearing loss. Tip link breakage is probably a protective measure, because too much glutamate release onto the cochlear nerve causes permanent cell death.
Place coding
Because of how the cochlea and basilar membrane are constructed, acoustic stimuli of different frequencies cause different amounts of movement along the basilar membrane. Higher frequency sounds cause bending of the basilar membrane closest to the stapes, resulting in more hair cell activity in that area. Moderate to high frequencies are entirely encoded by place coding. Human speech is in this frequency range.
Rate coding
Low frequency sounds are processed using a rate coding system: the pattern of neurotransmitter release from the hair cells deepest in the cochlea (furthest from the stapes) determine the perception of low frequency sounds.
Graph of three curves indicating sensitivity of inner hair cells
Low points of three solid curves indicate that these inner hair cells will respond to faint sound only if it is of a specific frequency. If the sound is louder, cells will respond to frequencies above and below their preferred frequencies. Lesions targeted to outer hair cells disrupt the responsiveness of inner hair cells to specific sounds.
Pitch perception
Moderate to high frequencies are encoded by place coding. Low frequencies are partly encoded by rate coding.
Loudness
Loudness corresponds to the total number of hair cells that are active and their overall activity levels.
Timbre
Timbre is perceived by assessing the precise mixture of hair cells that area active throughout the entire cochlea.
Fundamental frequency
The lowest and most intense frequency of a complex sound. This frequency is most often perceived as sound’s basic pitch.
Overtone
Sound wave frequencies that occurs at integer multiples of the fundamental frequency.
Timbre
The specific mixture of frequencies (fundamental frequency plus overtones) that different instruments emit when the same note is played. It is the complexity of the sound wave. We analyze the timbre of a sound and how the timbre changes over time to identify which instrument made the sound.
Cohlear implants
Cochlear implants for the hearing-impaired take advantage of the place coding system of the cochlea. They elicit the perception of different notes by stimulating different places along the cochlea (with 20 to 24 evenly spaced electrodes). Loudness is controlled by the frequency stimulation.
Interaural cues
Differences in sound perception between the two ears. One of the main ways we localize sounds is by analyzing the timing difference between the 2 ears (which ear heard the sound first).
Interaural loudness
For high frequency sounds (above 800Hz) we use intramural loudness differences to help identify the location of a sound (which ear heard it louder). This approach is possible because the loudness of a high-pitched (high-frequency) sound is significantly dampened by the head.
Localizing low frequency sounds
To help localize low frequency sounds (below 800 Hz, which corresponds to wavelengths that are longer than the width of the head), the brain analyzes the phase differences between the two ears.
Identifying the height of a sound
To discriminate whether a sound is coming in front, behind, or above you, we analyze the timbre of the sound wave. The shape of our outer ear (pinna and ear canal) creates a direction-selective filter; different frequencies are enhanced/attenuated when sound enters our ears from different directions. These effects are highly individual (depending on the shape and size of the outer ear). We are not born with this skill - must continuously learn.
From the Ear to Primary Auditory Cortex
- The organ of Corti sends auditory information to the brain via the cochlear nerve.
- These axons synapse in the cochlear nuclei of the medulla, where copies of the signal are made to be analyzed in parallel ascending paths.
- Axons from the cochlear nuclei synapse in the superior olivary nuclei in the medulla and the inferior colliculi in the midbrain, both of which help localize the source of sounds.
- Axons from the inferior colliculi synapse in the medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, which in turn relays the information to the…
- Primary auditory cortex in the temporal lobe
Tonotopic representation
Like the Basilian membrane, the primary auditory cortex respond best to different frequencies. In this organization different frequencies of sound are analyzed in different places of auditory cortex.
Primary auditory cortex (core region)
In the upper section of the temporal lobe, mostly hidden in the lateral fissure.
Auditory association cortex
The belt and parable regions refer to the auditory association cortex. Like visual information, auditory information is analyzed in “where” and “what” streams.
The posterior (dorsal) auditory pathway
Involved in sound localization. This pathway meets up with the “where” vision pathway in the parietal lobe.
Anterior auditory pathway
Important for recognizing what produced a sound (not the location of the sound). It is sometimes called the auditory object recognition pathway, and it extends from the temporal lobe into the frontal lobe.
Auditory Agnosia
Music and language are special, complex forms of auditory processing, and brain damage in auditory association cortex can cause very specific types of auditory agnosia. Different areas of auditory association cortex process the melody, rhythm, and harmony (overtones) of music. Other areas of auditory association cortex process the melody, rhythm, and harmony (overtones) of music. Other areas of auditory association cortex are involved in the perception of sound as pleasant (consonant) or unpleasant (dissonant), and certain combinations of musical notes can trigger emotions. There are many types of auditory agnosias.
Amusia
Amusia is the inability to perceive or produce melodic music. People with amuse might be unable to sing or recognize the happy birthday song. People with amusia can often converse and understand speech. They can also recognize environmental sounds. They can even recognize the emotions conveyed in music, but they will typically be unable to tell the difference between consonant music (pleasant sounding) and dissonant music (unstable) even though these sounds might alter their emotional state just as they do in other people.
Vestibular System
The inner ear beyond the cochlea. Vestibular system detects gravity and tilts and turns of the head. Cochlea detects sound. Vestibular system doesn’t always produce readily definable, conscious sensations. But it contributes to our sense of balance, maintains an upright head position, and corrects eye movements to compensate for head movements. Disruptions in the vestibular system can cause dizziness and nausea.
Otolith organs (vestibular sacs)
Two distinct structures: the utricle & saccule - that monitor the angle of the head and linear acceleration of the head.
Semicircular canals
Three ring-like, fluid-filled structures that detect changes in head rotation (angular acceleration). In each semicircular canal is one bulge called the ampulla, where a gelatinous mass (cupula) pulls open hair cells in response to movement of fluid in the canals.
Otolith Organs
In the otolith organs (the utricle & saccule), it is the weight of a stone of calcium carbonate (an otolith) that pulls open hair cell ion channels to indicate the angle of the head and whether it is accelerating along a linear path. One otolith sits in the horizontal plane, and the other sits in the vertical plane.
Somatosensory System
Provides information about touch, pressure, temperature, and pain, both on the surface of the skin and inside the body.
Three interacting somatosensory systems
- Exteroceptive system (cutaneous/skin senses): responds to external stimuli applied to the skin (e.g., touch and temperature).
- The interoceptive system (organic senses): provides information about conditions within the body and is responsible for efficient regulation of its internal milieu (e.g., heart rate, breathing, hunger, bladder).
- The proprioceptive (kinesthesia) system: monitors information about the position of the body, posture, and movement (e.g., the tension of the muscles inside the body).
The Cutaneous Senses
The cutaneous senses (skin) encode different types of external stimuli:
1. Pressure (touch) is caused by mechanical deformation of the skin
2. Vibrations occur when we move our fingers across a rough surface
3. Temperature is produced by objects that heat or cool the skin
4. Pain can be caused by many different types of stimuli, but primarily tissue damage.
Epidermis
Outer most layer of the skin. (“above dermis”). Cells here get oxygen from the air (not the blood).
Dermis
Middle layer of the skin.
Hypodermis or Subcutaneous
The deepest layer of the skin. (“below the skin”). Sensory neurons are scattered throughout these layers.
Merkel’s disks
Respond to local skin indentations (simple touch).
Ruffini Corpuscles
Are sensitive to stretch and the kinaesthetic sense of finger position and movement.
Pacinian corpuscles
Respond to skin vibrations
Glabrous skin
Hairless skin. E.g., palms of hands and feet.
Free nerve endings
Primarily respond to temperature and pain
Meissner’s corpuscles
Only found in glabrous skin. They detect very light touch and localized edge contours (brail-like stimuli).
Temperature / two categories of thermal receptors
Two categories of thermal receptors: those that respond to warmth and those that respond to coolness. Pain information is also conveyed by some of these cells. This information is poorly localized, and the axons that carry it to the CNS are unmyelinated or thinly myelinated. Some of the receptor proteins that are sensitive to temperature can also be activated by certain ligands.
Pain
Sensations of pain and temperature are transducer by free nerve endings in the skin. There are several types of pain receptor cells (usually referred to as nociceptors - “detectors of noxious stimuli”).