Memory Flashcards
Coding
refers to the format or ‘type’ of information which is stored in each memory store. Coding is acoustic in short-term memory, and semantic in long-term memory, as demonstrated by Baddeley (1966), who found that more mistakes are made when recalling acoustically-similar words straight after learning them, whilst more mistakes are made when recalling semantically-similar words 20 minutes after learning them (LTM recall).
Capacity
refers to the volume of information/data which can be kept in any memory store at any one time. For example, the capacity of STM is thought to be 7 +/- 2 items (Miller), whilst the capacity of LTM is unlimited. This is based on Miller’s idea that things come in groups of 7 (e.g. 7 days of the week), suggesting that we are predisposed to remembering this quantity and that such a ‘chunking’ method can help us recall information. Jacobs also demonstrated that the mean letter span was 7.3 and the mean digit span was 9.3 (i.e. the number of letters or digits we can recall after increasing intervals).
Duration
refers to the amount of time that information can be stored in each memory store. The duration of STM is about18, as demonstrated by Petersen et al (1959), who found that increasing retention intervals decreased the accuracy of recall of consonant syllables in 24 undergraduates, when counting down from a 3 digit number (preventing mental rehearsal). The duration of LTM is unlimited, as shown by Bahrick et al (1975), who found that photo recognition of graduating classmates of the 396 participants decreased from 90% to 70% between 15 years and 46 years of graduating.
Atkinson and Shiffrin’s Multi store model
*-There are 3 stores: the sensory register, short-term memory and long-term memory.
-The sensory register contains one sub-store for
each of the 5 senses e.g. an echoic store for
auditory information. Since it receives information
from our senses, the sensory register has a huge
capacity, but a duration of less than half a second. Therefore, information will only pass from the sensory register to the short-term memory store if we pay attention to it.
STM- has a duration of 18 seconds. capacity is 7±2 items, and is acoustically coded.
LTM is coded semantically. has an unlimited duration and capacity.
Coding, duration and capacity of STM
- STM is described as being acoustically encoded (Baddeley), having a capacity of 7+/- 2 items
(Miller) and a duration of 18-30 seconds (Petersen). Maintenance rehearsal occurs when we repeat
the new information to ourselves, allowing the information to be kept in the STM. Prolonged
maintenance rehearsal allows the information to pass into the LTM, whilst a lack of such rehearsal
causes forgetting.
Coding, duration and capacity of LTM
LTM is described as being semantically encoded, having an unlimited capacity and a very long
duration (over 46 years, as shown by Bahrick et al). In order to remember information, ‘retrieval’
must occur, which is when information is transferred back into the STM, and will continue to pass
through the maintenance loop afterwards.
Types of LTM: Episodic
Refers to our ability to recall events (episodes) in our lives and consists of memories such as our thoughts or experiences we have had and our personal recollections of them. These memories have some kind of personal meaning to us, alongside details as to when and how these events occurred, as well as the associated people and places. An example would be the memory of a wedding or the first time meeting a partner.
You have to make a conscious effort to recall episodic memories.
Types of LTM: Semantic
Contains knowledge, facts, concepts and meanings the individual has learnt e.g. the capital of France is Paris. Semantic memory may also relate to how certain objects work, their functions, appropriate behaviour in situations or abstract concepts such as language or mathematics. These are not time-stamped and are typically not personal to us.
Types of LTM: Procedural
This is our memory for actions, skills, or basically how we do things. Procedural memories are usually learnt through repetition and practice and do not require conscious thought.
The working memory model
Is an explanation of how STM is organised and how it functions. The working memory model replaced the idea of a unitary Short-term memory store (STM) and suggested a system involving multiple stores consisting of active processing and short-term storage of information. The WMM suggests that STM is made up of the central executive, the phonological loop, the visuospatial sketchpad and the episodic buffer
WMM: The central executive
The main component and coordinates the other “slave systems” and ensures they don’t go astray. This has a very limited processing capacity.
WMM: The phonological loop
Acts like the “Inner ear” and processes auditory information so is coded acoustically.
The capacity is believed to be about 2 seconds.
The PL is divided into:
-Phonological store stores the words you hear.
-Articulatory process acts as the “Inner Voice” linked to speech production and is used to rehearse and store verbal information from the phonological store through maintenance rehearsal.
WMM: The visuo-spatial sketchpad
Processes visual information through the senses (eyes) or long-term memory on what things look like, patterns of recognition and spatial information. This has a limited capacity of about three or four objects (Baddeley).
Logie (1995) suggested the VSS could be further subdivided into:
-Visual cache which stores visual material on colour and form
-Inner scribe which deals with spatial relationships, rehearsal and the transfer of information from the visual cache to the central executive.
WMM: The Episodic Buffer
Integrates all types of data processed by the other stores and so is described as the storage component of the central executive, as well as being crucial for linking STM to LTM. The episodic buffer has a limited capacity of about 4 chunks and also maintains time-sequencing recording events as they happen and transferring this information into long-term memory.
Explanations for forgetting: Interference
Interference occurs when the recall of one memory blocks the recall of another, causing forgetting or
distorted perceptions of these memories.
Proactive interference occurs when an older memory interferes with a new one
Retroactive interference occurs when a newer memory interferes with an old one.
Research on similarity: interference
In both PI and RI, interference is worse when words are similar.
Mcgeoch and Mcdonald studied RETROACTIVE interference by changing the amount of similarity between two sets of material. Participants had to learn a set of 10 words until they could remember them all with 100% accuracy. They then learned a new list. there were six groups of participants who had to learn different types of new lists:
group 1=synonyms to the originals
group 2=antonyms to the originals
group 3=words unrelated to the original ones
group 4=consonant syllables
group 5= three-digit numbers
group 6=no new list-they just rested (the control group
When the participants were asked to recall the original list of words, the synonyms produced the worst recall, which shows that recall is worse when words are similar.
Explanations for forgetting: retrieval failure
Cues act as markers to aid recall and without these, the mind is unable to locate the correct memory. Retrieval failure suggests that forgetting occurs when the ‘cues’ (triggers of information recollection) present at the time of encoding the information are not present at the time of recall. This describes Tulving’s ‘encoding specificity principle’ (ESP).
* Context-dependent forgetting occurs when our external cues at the time of encoding do not match
those present at recall (eg location). This was demonstrated by Godden and Baddeley (1975) who found that with deep-water divers, recall at the ‘matching’ conditions (e.g. word list learnt underwater and recalled underwater) was significantly larger than the non-matching conditions. Therefore, there were 4 conditions in total, involving underwater and on-land encoding and recall.
* State-dependent forgetting occurs when our internal cues at the time of encoding do not match
those present at recall. This was demonstrated by Carter and Cassaday (1998), who gave anti-histamines instead to change the internal cue at the time of encoding and recall ( drowsy). The researchers found 40% higher rates of accurate recall in the matching conditions, compared to the non-matching conditions.
Factors affecting EWT: Misleading information
Loftus and Palmer leading questions
In the Loftus and Palmer study, 45 students were shown several films of road traffic incidents and then given a questionnaire to describe the accident and answer a series of questions about their observation. One critical question varied between conditions with one group asked how fast the vehicles were going when they “hit” each other while other groups had verbs implying different degrees of a collision such as “bumped, smashed, contacted, collided”.Results found the words that implied a stronger collision resulted in greater average estimates of speeds from participants.
Those exposed to “smashed” gave the highest estimates (41mph) while “contacted” resulted in the lowest speed estimate (30mph), demonstrating how leading questions could influence memory recall.
The experiment was recreated with another group with the verbs “smashed” and “hit” while a control group was not exposed to such leading questions. They were questioned 1 week later and asked a series of questions with one critical question being whether they witnessed any broken glass.
There was no broken glass in the film however results found that those who were exposed to the “smashed” condition and thus led to believe the car was travelling faster were more likely to report seeing broken glass with the control group being the least likely.
Factors affecting EWT: Misleading info
Gabbert et al Post event discussion
Gabbert et al (2003) conducted a study where participants were paired up and each partner watched a different POV video of the same event so that they had each viewed unique items. Pairs in one condition were encouraged to discuss the event before each partner recalled the event they watched.
The results found that 71% of witnesses who had discussed the event went on to mistakenly recall items acquired during their discussion, compared to the 0% control group rate who had worked alone throughout.
Post-event discussions also demonstrate the idea of ‘memory conformity’, where we are more likely to pick up upon incorrect ideas or details because we believe that we are wrong and the other person is right.