Lecture 8: Attitudes About Others Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

need to evaluate (5)

A
  • Our tendency to engage in evaluation;
  • Describe events from previous day;
  • Use more positive or negative words (e.g. friendly, mean);
  • And have more accessible attitudes towards everyday objects (e.g. butterfly, spinach).
  • However, the Need to Evaluate Scale (Jarvis & Petty, 1996) was developed to measure our attitudes towards things, not people.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Tormala & Petty (2001) (10)

A
  • Found that the need to evaluate produces differences in our tendency to evaluate others.
    • High: Engage in careful evaluation after each stimulus.
    • Low: Prone to make a global judgement in the moment it’s required.
  • Participants read 20 sentences about a hypothetical person, half good and half bad.
  • Completed attitude (if the person is good/bad) & recall (as many things as possible about person), and individual difference measures (need to evaluate).
  • On-Line: Form impression of target; will be asked later.
  • Memory-Based: Note how complex sentences are.
  • For on-line condition and high need to evaluate: No association between recall valence & attitudes towards target.
  • For memory-based condition and low need to evaluate: More positive recall index → more positive attitude.
  • Problem: Situations where this is not the case (e.g. more realistic or personally relevant scenarios).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

attitudes from stereotypes (3)

A
  • The first things we notice about others is their gender, race, and age.
  • We tend to assume people are representative of the group we perceive them to be in.
  • In other words, our beliefs about someone’s group become part of our attitude toward that person.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Locksley et al. (1980) (6)

A
  • Found that having more information than just gender reduces stereotype effects.
  • Participants read about 6 students (3M, 3F), varying the description provided, all relating to assertiveness.
    • Control: no scenario given (name only).
    • Nondiagnostic description: a situation that had nothing to do with assertiveness (e.g. going for a haircut).
    • Diagnostic description: a situation regarding assertiveness (e.g. having an in-class debate).
  • After reading the diagnostic description of a student, participants rated him/her as equally assertive (regardless of the gender).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Kunda & Sherman-Williams (1993) (6)

A
  • Participants read about a construction worker or a housewife who “hit someone who annoyed him/her.”
  • Were given info that indicated high or low aggression, or no info.
  • Asked how aggressive he/she is.
  • When participants were given either unambiguous description of the behaviour, they ignored the stereotype.
  • But when the behaviour was ambiguous, the construction worker was viewed as more aggressive.
  • Problem: Were the construction worker and housewife really viewed the same? Were stereotypes really not activated?
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Kunda et al. (1997) (9)

A
  • Did a follow-up to Kunda & Sherman-Williams (1993).
  • Participants read about John, who walked away from someone taunting him without saying anything (low aggression).
  • Half told John is a construction worker, other half told he is an accountant.
  • Both conditions rated John as equally unaggressive.
  • But participants in construction worker condition rated John as more likely to engage in other stereotypically working-class aggressive behaviours (e.g. get into bar fights, make rude comments at women).
  • Demonstrates that expectations can be influenced by other aspects of the stereotype that remain unchallenged.
    • Individuating information only goes so far.
    • Specific, unambiguous individuating information can undercut effect of stereotypes for judgments that directly relate to the individuating information.
    • But any stereotypes that are activated can still influence formation of one’s overall concept & collection of beliefs about that person.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

stereotype content model (SCM) or Fiske et al. (2002)

A
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

consequences of intergroup bias (9)

A
  • Information processing: Stereotypes can influence how we interpret others’ behaviour & events.
  • Can also affect our behaviour towards those groups, via:
    • Blatant discrimination resulting in significant disadvantages and severe emotional and/or physical harm;
    • Subtle/aversive discrimination (e.g. benevolent sexism);
    • Limiting our interaction with outgroup members.
  • Our behaviour can, in turn, affect outgroup members’ behaviour.
  • Attitudes can also determine how we interpret social situations, how we treat people, and how they respond.
    • In turn influences who we get to know better and what info we’re exposed to.
    • Thus, many people are motivated to change their negative attitudes toward others.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Word et al. (1974) (5)

A
  • White participants interviewed White and Black job applicants (confederates).
  • Interviewers treated applicants differently regarding: physical distance, speech errors, and interview length.
  • Follow-up study where White confederate interviewers gave people the “White treatment” or “Black treatment”.
  • Other participants viewed tapes of the interviews and rated those who got the Black treatment as less qualified for the job.
  • Shows how stereotypes can influence behaviour of the person holding the stereotype and, consequently, the person being stereotyped (thereby reinforcing the original stereotype).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Macrae et al. (1994) (6)

A
  • Found that we can consciously suppress stereotypes, but this may lead to more intense activation and application later on.
  • Participants shown photo of a male skinhead, asked to write a description of a typical day in his life.
  • Half instructed to suppress stereotype of skinheads; controls were not.
  • Descriptions written by the suppression group were less stereotype-consistent.
  • However, on a subsequent lexical decision task, suppressed participants were faster to respond to stereotypic words.
    • Also, when told that they were going to meet the skinhead from the photo, suppression participants sat further away from the chair with the person’s jacket.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

contact hypothesis (4)

A
  • Contact must be among people of equal status in pursuit of common goals (Allport, 1954).
  • Further research indicated that we also need:
    • Opportunities to get to know each other;
    • Exposure to evidence that disconfirms stereotypes;
    • And active cooperation.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Shook & Fazio (2008) (6)

A
  • Can having a Black roommate reduce implicit bias?
  • White 1st years randomly assigned to share a room with a Black student or White student.
  • Assessed level of satisfaction with roommate & implicit prejudice (using the evaluative priming measure) at begining & end of term.
  • White students with Black roommates reported lower roommate satisfaction scores at both sessions.
  • But also scored lower on implicit prejudice in 2nd session.
  • Conclusion: Increased contact led to more positive implicitly measured attitudes, despite less (explicit) satisfaction.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

when we don’t disconfirm stereotypes (4)

A
  • When we encounter individuals that disconfirm stereotypes, can hang onto stereotype by creating exception/subtype.
  • Particularly likely when:
    • All disconfirming examples have another shared attribute.
    • Individuals are consistent with stereotype in other ways.
    • Stereotype violation is more extreme.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

when we do disconfirm stereotypes (4)

A
  • Stereotypes can change over time, as people encounter more and more people who:
    • Moderately violate the stereotype;
    • Violate the stereotype in different and/or multiple ways.
  • parasocial contact: e.g. Knowing friends who have outgroup friends.
  • And also by forming a close relationship with people of outgroups before knowing they are a member of an outgroup.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Onaret et al. (2007) (4)

A
  • Found that low trait emotional intelligence (EI) correlates with higher Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA), Social Dominance Orientation (SDO), and subtle racial rejudice.
  • Participants completed questionnaires measuring their trait EI, RWA, SDO, and radical prejudice.
  • Because low trait EI correlates with lack of perspective taking, this explains why individuals are more prejudiced.
  • Low trait EI also correlates with seeing stressful events as threatening, explaining the relationship to RWA.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Fein & Spencer (1997) (9)

A
  • Study 1: Participants evaluated a target who was either a member of a group with an available negative stereotype (Jewish) or not (Italian) on her personality and qualification for a job.
    • Before evaluating the target, participants were self-affirmed or not.
    • Not affirmed participants evaluated the Jewish target’s personality and qualification significantly more negatively than in their other evaluations.
  • Study 2: Participants evaluated men as either gay or straight after receiving self-image-threatening info or not (score on bogus IQ test, told it was real or not real).
    • Participants were more likely to evaluate the target consistently with the gay stereotype after receiving threatening info.
  • Study 3: All participants were given negative or positive feedback from a bogus IQ test (told was real), had their state self-esteem measured, then evaluated a Jewish or Italian woman, then measured state self-esteem again.
    • Participants rated the target’s personality based on ethnicity if they received negative feedback.
    • Participants who received negative feedback and evaluated the Jewish target had a significantly greater increase in state self-esteem than other participants.
    • Suggests that these participants restored their self-esteem by engaging in negative evaluation of the stereotyped target.