Lecture 2: Indirect Measurement, Attitude Functions, & Attitude Strength Flashcards
indirect measurement (1)
- Based on the assumption that stronger (therefore more accessible) associations activate evaluations quickly, which then influences the speed at which we respond.
implicit association test (IAT) (3)
- Respondents classify adjectives and attitude objects as fast as possible.
- A faster reaction time indicates a stronger attitude.
- Gained widespread popularity, used for a variety of things.
problems with the IAT (4)
- IAT is based off the assumption that responses can’t be controlled, and is less susceptible to outside influence than direct measurement.
- But people can exert some control over the IAT (e.g. purposely going slower) and context matters (e.g. showing liked pictures of black people before a Black-White IAT);
- It’s unclear how much respondents scores reflect their own attitudes vs. their knowledge of others’ attitudes.
- IAT only measures a relative not absolute favorability.
physiological indirect measures (3)
- The galvanic skin response (GSR) and pupillary dilation are two methods that don’t rely on response time.
- Facial EMG, ERPs, and fMRIs.
- Problem: more sensitive to strength than valence.
facial electromyography (facial EMG) (2)
- Assesses contractions of facial muscles, which have a distinct pattern of activation for true vs. fake emotions (e.g. a smile).
- Problem: difficult to administer, expensive, and difficult to analyze the data.
event-related potentials (ERPs) (2)
- Measures electrical activity in the brain.
- Helped us understand the time course in which individuals make attitudinal judgments.
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (2)
- Shows where blood and oxygen are in the brain, i.e. activity.
- Helped us understand what brain regions are active when recalling/reporting an attitude.
Fazio, Blasovich, & Driscoll (1992) (7)
- Provided evidence for the functionality of attitudes.
- The strength of association in memory between an attitude object and one’s evaluation determines how accessible it is.
- Strong, i.e. highly accessible, attitudes will guide our decisions in a spontaneous manner.
- Attitudes help us make quick decisions because we need less resources.
- And decisions that are higher in quality, i.e. less likely to change at a later time.
- However, the benefits of attitudes may hold true for any construct that people might form (e.g. self-knowledge).
- Because attitudes are automatic and not likely to change, they can also make a person more close-minded.
reliability (1)
- The degree to which scores are free from errors in measurement.
internal consistency reliability (1)
- Asks: are the individual items assessing the same thing?
test-retest reliability (1)
- Asks: are the scores consistent over time?
validity (1)
- The extent that a measurement assesses the construct it was designed to measure.
convergent validity (1)
- Asks: does it align/correlate with other measures?
discriminant validity (1)
- Asks: is it uncorrelated with measures of other, irrelevant constructs?
predictive validity (1)
- Asks: does it predict future behaviour?
reliability and validity of explicit measures (2)
- High in reliability: the more items the better (if they’re similar), but even single item semantic differential scales have been shown to have high test-retest reliability.
- Often valid, when there isn’t a distinct socially acceptable answer.
- Is higher when assessing simple attitude objects.
reliability and validity of implicit measures (2)
- Appears to have high reliability, but it hasn’t been tested (e.g. the IAT has high test-retest reliability).
- High convergent validity, and good predictive validity.
- Is generally higher when assessing simple attitude objects.
relative strength (1)
- How much you favour/disfavour one attitude object relative to another, and is often what’s measured (explicitly and implicitly).
absolute strength (1)
- How certain someone is of their attitude and how important it is to them.
characteristics of strong attitudes (4)
- Persistent over time.
- Resistant to change.
- Likely to influence information processing.
- Likely to predict behaviour.
Katz (1960) (1)
- Explored the primary and secondary functions of attitudes; i.e. object appraisal, utilitarian, value-expressive, and ego-defensive.
object appraisal (knowledge) function (2)
- States that attitudes let us know whether we like or dislike an object so that we don’t have to make a new judgement every time.
- Some objects are more likely to fulfill only one function (e.g. air conditioner: utilitarian) whereas others fulfill multiple functions (e.g. handbags: utilitarian and value-expressive).
utilitarian (instrumental) function (1)
- To approach things that meet our needs and maximize rewards, and to avoid punsihments and things that thwart our needs.
value-expressive function (1)
- To reflect our central values and self-image/concept.
ego-defensive function (1)
- To bolster or protect our self-esteem.
problems with attitude functions (2)
- Ambiguity in distinction between different functions.
- Accessing functions largely requires conscious awareness, which is particularly problematic for ego-defensive functions because they’re most effective when a person is unaware of them.
Pillaud et al. (2013) (9)
- Attitudinal ambivalence isn’t simply a weak attitude but is actually adaptive in social interactions.
- Participants used an open-ended measure to convey their attitudes towards a controversial topic (GMOs) and a non-controversial topic (tooth brushing).
- They did this in a control condition, and conditions where they were self-enhancing or self-depreciating.
- Expressed more ambivalence towards GMOs in the self-enhancing vs. self-depreciating condition.
- Expressed more ambivalence towards tooth brushing in the self-depreciating vs. self-enhancing condition.
- Expressed less ambivalence towards tooth brushing in the control and self-enhancing conditions vs. GMOs.
- If you show ambivalence towards a controversial topic, it can show that you’re unbiased and more intelligent and also protect you from judgement.
- You shouldn’t show ambivalence towards a non-controversial topic because there’s a socially accepted right answer, and not knowing it makes you deviant/uneducated about the society.
- Certain boundaries exist to this rule, e.g. if there’s a socially acceptable answer towards a controversial topic.