Lecture 6: Motor development Flashcards
Reflexes in newborns
Newborns demonstrate reflexes – innate, fixed patterns of action that occur in response to particular stimulation
Not always entirely automatic Most reflexes fade during the first few months and long persistence can used in diagnosing motor disorders
Some reflexes, such as rooting and sucking, have a clear adaptive (survival) value
Others, such as the tonic neck reflex, have no clear adaptive significance
What are the key feeding reflexes
Rooting: Baby turns her head and opens her mouth when her cheek is stroked Disappears at around 3 weeks when is replaced by voluntary head turning
Sucking: when something is put in her mouth, the baby sucks repeatedly
Replaced by voluntary sucking at around 4 months
Grasping reflexes in the hand
When a finger or object is pressed against the baby’s palm, the baby’s fingers close around it in a grasping action
Disappears by around 4 months when it is replaced with voluntary grasping
Grasping reflexes in the foot
Appears in the foot as well as the hand (evolutionary remnant)
Human toes are too short for proper grasping
Reflex in hands and feet may once have helped baby cling on to (hairy) mother – the way nonhuman primates still do today
Babinski Reflexes
When the bottom of baby’s foot is stroked, the toes fan out and then curl
Disappears around 8- 12 months
Long-term persistence might indicate neurological problems
Stepping reflex
When baby is held upright over a flat surface, s/he will make rhythmic stepping movements
Disappears around 2 months
Moro reflex
If baby is startled by something (e.g. sudden noise or movement) then baby throws arms out backwards and arches back before bringing arms back together as if holding something
Disappears around 6 months
Long-term persistence might indicate neurological problems
Development of the cortex
Early, simple reflexes arise from the brain stem
More complex, coordinated reflexes result from the maturation of the cerebral cortex
Primary motor cortex (M1)
First area of the cortex to develop
Responsible for voluntary (non-reflexive) movement
Begins with raising head (1month), control of arms and trunk (3 months); leg control is last to develop
Why study motor processes
Why do we have a brain
To produce adaptable and complex movement
The only way you have to affect the world around you (with one exception)
Motor development
Previously believed to be an element of neurological maturity
Current theories, however, often take a dynamic systems approach, emphasizing a confluence of many factors
Not only neural mechanisms but also increases in strength, posture control, balance, perceptual skills, and motivation
Illustrating the dynamic systems view
Research by Esther Thelen and colleagues examined the stepping reflex, the infant’s performance of stepping movements when he or she is held under the arms with feet touching a surface.
The reflex was thought to disappear at about 2 months of age because of cortical maturation, but demonstrations that the reflex could be prolonged or elicited long after it was scheduled to disappear were inconsistent with this interpretation.
Testing the dynamic systems view
Thelen et al., performed two experiments to test the hypothesis that rapid increases in infants’ weight made it impossible for them to execute stepping motions.
In one experiment, weights were attached to the ankles of infants who still had the stepping reflex, and the babies suddenly stopped stepping.
In the second study, infants who no longer showed the stepping reflex were found to do so when they were suspended waist-deep in a tank of water that supported their weight.
Therefore, the movement pattern and its neural basis remained but was masked by the changing ratio of leg weight to strength.
Impact of culture on motor development
Mothers in Mali believe it is important to exercise their infants to promote their physical and motor development.
The maneuvers shown here do not harm the babies and do hasten their early motor skills (Bril & Sabatier, 1986)
Reaching
For the first few months, infants are limited to prereaching movements
clumsy swiping movements by young infants toward the general vicinity of objects they see
Infants begin successfully reaching for objects at around 3 to 4 months of age