Lecture 5: Language Development Flashcards
SPEECH PERCEPTION
High Amplitude Sucking Procedure
- Used to test infants from birth to 4 months of age
- Relies on infants sucking reflex
- Infants hear a sound stimulus every time they produce a strong/ high-amplitude suck on a pacifier –> child quickly learns that strong suck produces noise.
- The number of strong sucks is an indicator of the infant’s interest
- More strong sucks = more interest
- 2 variations of procedure:
- Discrimination
- Preference
High Amplitude Sucking Procedure - Discrimination (similar to habituation)
Discrimination:
* Used to test whether infants can tell the difference between two auditory stimuli
* Variation of visual habituation paradigm
* Habituation phase: Each time infant produces a strong suck, a sound is played
- Continues until sucking has declined significantly (e.g. by 20%)
* Test phase: Hears new speech stimulus every time produces a strong suck
* If can distinguish between stimuli, sucking behaviour should increase –> expect dishabituation = increase in sucking
High Amplitude Sucking Procedure - Preference
Preference:
* Used to test infants’ preference for different stimuli
* 2 different stimuli are played on alternating minutes each time a strong suck is produced
- i.e., minute 1 = Stimulus A, minute 2 = Stimulus B, minute 3 = Stimulus A
* Number of strong sucks produced during presentation of each stimulus type is compared
* Preference = infants suck more during one stimulus minute type than the other –> this shows preference for this stimuli
Preferential Listening Procedure
- Speaker on either side of infant’s head (speakers blink at the start to orient children to understand where noise is coming from)
- When looks at speaker, a recording of speech plays
- Different speech from each speaker
- How long an infant spends looking in a particular direction/ listening to a particular sound indicates how much they like it
- Familiarity effect: Will listen longer to sounds they recognize
- Novelty effect: If first habituated to a sound, will listen longer to new sound
Note: only used with 4 months old and older - needs to be able to control neck movement
Speech Perception in Infancy
- Using high amplitude sucking paradigms, research has shown that newborns:
- Prefer to listen to speech sounds over artificial/electronic sounds
- Prefer mother’s voice over another woman’s voice
- Prefer to listen to native language vs. other language
- Suggests that language learning starts in utero - starts before birth!
Categorical Perception of Speech
- Adults perceive speech sounds as distinct categories even though the differences between speech sounds is gradual
- The way the 2 sounds are produced by our mouth is the same (super similar). They just have different voice onset times (VOT) (b/ vs p/).
- Anything that has a shorter VOT than 25 is perceived as b/ anything longer than 25 VOT is perceived as p/. There is a continuum but we dont hear that we hear 2 distinct categories.
Categorical Perception of Speech
- Categorical perception is useful because focuses listeners on sounds that are linguistically meaningful while ignoring meaningless differences
* E.g. difference between a 10ms VOT /b/ vs. 20ms VOT /b/ is meaningless in English
Infant Categorical Perception of Speech
- Do infants perceive the same speech categories as adults?
- Classic study by Eimaset al., 1971:
- Tested 1 month old infants learning English
- Used High amplitude sucking paradigm to test discrimination between /ba/ and /pa/
-
2 groups:
- Different speech sounds: Infants habituated to /ba/ (20 msVOT) and then tested with /pa/ (40 msVOT)
- Same speech sounds: Infants habituated to 60 msVOT /pa/ and then tested with 80 msVOT /pa
If infants can distinguish between ba and pa, how should they behave?
Increase sucking when sound from new category (‘pa’)
Infant Categorical Perception of Speech
- Newborns have same categorical perception of speech as adult
- They seem to have a similar category boundary as adults.
Results:
* Increased sucking when sound from new category.
* No change in sucking when sound from same category.
* Conclude that at one month old, we have the same categorical perception as adults (innate).
Infant Cross-Language Speech Perception
- Infants make more distinctions between speech sounds than adults
- Adults have difficulty perceiving differences between speech sounds that are not important in their native language
- E.g. In French, difference between /ou/ and /u/ is meaningful, but not in English
- What about infants?
Infant Cross-Language Speech Perception
- Classic study by Werker et al., 1988:
- Tested 6 month olds American infants learning English
- High amplitude sucking paradigm to see if they can discriminate between Hindi /Ta/ and /ta/
- English speaking adults struggle to distinguish between these 2 sounds
Infants Cross-Language Speech Perception (results)
Results:
* After habituating to one of these Hindi speech sounds, increased sucking
when heard other speech sounds
* i.e., if habituated to /Ta/, then increased sucking when tested with /ta/
Implications
- Infants discriminate between speech sounds they have never heard
before (i.e., speech sounds not found in their native language) - Infants are biologically ready to learn any of the world’s languages
Perceptual Narrowing of Speech Perception
- Infants’ ability to easily distinguish between non-native speech sounds diminishes around 8 months
- Not impossible to learn to distinguish betweeen these sounds but just harder.
- Perceptual narrowing is not a bad thing, its a good thing, makes the perception of these sounds more efficient. - By 10-12 months, infants’ perceptual abilities are narrowed to those sounds that are relevant to their native language
- Improves perception of speech sounds in native language
Word Segmentation
-
Word segmentation: knowing where words begin and end in fluent speech
- Begins around 7 months of age
- Infants’ statistical learning enables them to segment words in a stream of speech (enables them to pick out words from a stream of speech)
- Stress-patterning
- Distribution of speech sounds
Stress Patterning
- Different languages place stress on different parts of a word
- English: stress usually on first syllable
- French: stress usually on last syllable (babies growing up in french household will learn that the stress on last syllable indicates end of a word).
- Infants pick up on the stress patterning in their language and use it as clues to for word segmentation
Distribution of Speech Sounds
Babies learn that:
* Sounds that appear together often are likely to be words
* Sounds that don’t appear together often are more likely to be boundaries between words.
* Example: “happy baby”
- “ba” and “by” occur together often because make word “baby”
- “ha” and “ppy” occur together often because make word “happy”
- “ppy” and “ba” occur together less often because don’t make a word and many different words can come before “baby” (“happy”, “little” ) and many words can come after “happy” (“birthday”, “baby”, “puppy”)
Distribution of Speech Sounds (study)
- Study: Preferential listening procedure (turn their head towards sound they prefer)
-
Habituation: 8-month-olds listened to a stream of syllables for a long time (2 mins)
- Some syllables always occurred together
- Others rarely or never occurred together - Test: Presented with a syllable sequence that always co-occurred (“tokibu”) vs. syllable sequences that rarely co-occurred (“bagopi”)
- none of these were real words to ensure they had never heard it before.
-
Habituation: 8-month-olds listened to a stream of syllables for a long time (2 mins)
Distribution of Speech Sounds (Results)
- Results: Listened longer to rarely occurring sequence
- Shows that infants understood word boundaries by detecting the likelihood of syllables belonging together
Summary of Speech Perception
- Speech perception is studied with high amplitude sucking procedure and preferential listening paradigm
- Infants have remarkable speech perception abilities
- From birth, show adult-like categorical perception of speech for sounds that are physically similar and able to distinguish between speech sounds not found in their native language
- Speech perception is narrowed to sounds found in native language by 10-12 months of age
- Infants are sensitive to the patterns of language and use it to segment words from speech beginning around 7 months of age
THE JOURNEY TO FIRST WORDS
Certain important milestones!
* Important as indicator of developmental delays
* Can give information to parents.
Cooing
- Start around 2 months of age
- Drawn out vowel sounds, like “ooooohhh” and “aahhhh”
- Helps infants gain motor control over their vocalizations
- Elicits reactions from caregivers leading to back-and-forth cooing with caregivers
- Serves as social function - elicits reaction from caregivers, mimics what a conversation is like.
Babbling
- Start around 7 months of age (6-10 months of age)
- Repetitive consonant-vowel syllables, like “papapa” and “babababa”
- Speech sounds not necessarily from native language
- Infant babbling is very similar across languages
-
Manual babbling: Deaf infants that are exposed to sign language babble with repetitive hand movements made up of pieces of full signs
- Deaf children do not verbally babble
- Evidence that language exposure is critical for babbling
babling includes consonants and vowels, cooing is just vowels
Functions of babbling
-
Social function: Practicing turn-taking in a dialogue
- Infant babbling elicits caregiver reactions which in turn elicit more babbling -
Learning function: Signal that the infant is alert and ready to learn
- Infants learn more when an adult labels a new object just after they babble vs. learning the word in the absence of babbling
- caregiver tells the new word after the baby has babbled = more likely for them to learn that word than if the caregiver tells them after they have not babbled.
Understanding the words precedes production
- Infants appear to understand high-frequency words around 6 months of age
- they start to understand words before they learn to produce them.
- In lab studies, when 6-month-olds are presented with pictures of common items and hear one of the pictures being named, they look to the correct picture more often than chance- But cannot yet name these items themselves
- Shows that infants understand more words than they can produce
- Shows that infants understand more words than their caregivers realize
First words
- First words are produced around 12 months of age (10-15 months)
- First word: Any specific utterance/vocalization consistently used to refer to a particular meaning or thing.
- Can be tricky to identify:
- Babbling can sound like words (babbling can sound like a first word - they arent actually referring to their mom)
- E.g. “mamamama”
- Meaning of a first word can differ from its standard meaning
- E.g. “woof woof” referring to “dog”
- they are not using the word dog but they have come up with some other label to say dog.
Mispronunciations of First Words
- Often mispronounced in predictable ways:
- Omit difficult parts of words:
- “Banana” –> “nana” - Substitute difficult sounds for easier sounds:
- “Rabbit” –> “wabbit” - Re-order sounds to put easy sound first
- “Spaghetti” –> “pisketti”
Earliest words in 3 languages
Bold = 10 most common words in these 3 languages
First Words
- Usually refer to family members, pets, or important objects (things of interest to the baby)
- Meaning of first words are very similar across cultures
- Suggests that infants around the world have similar interests and priorities
Limitations of First Words
- Infants express themselves initially with only one-word utterances so cannot clearly communicate what they want to say
-
Overextension: using a word in a broader context than is appropriate
- E.g. “dog” refers to any 4 legged animal
- Does not mean that they don’t understand what the word refers to
-
Underextension: using a word in a more limited context than appropriate
- “cat” only refers to the family’s pet cat
Learning more words
-
18 months of age:
- Knows about 50 words
- Vocabulary spurt: Rate of word learning accelerates
WORD LEARNING