humour Flashcards
what is humour?
- humour is a psychological response characterized by the positive emotion of amusement, the appraisal that something is funny, and the tendency to laugh
- derives from the study of medicine of the ancient Greeks which taught that the balance of fluids in the human body, known as ‘humours’ controlled human health and emotion
sense of humour
- the ability to perceive what is ludicrous, or amusing
- or of expressing it in speech, writing or another composition
4 essential components of humour
- social context
- cognitive - perceptual processes
- emotional response
- vocal-behavioural expression of laughter
social context
- We laugh/joke much more when we are with others than by ourselves
- Provides us with opportunities for play
–> serves important social, emotional and cognitive functions
cognitive - perceptual processes
- Involves an idea, image, text, or event that is in some sense absurd, surprising, or out of the ordinary
- We need to cognitively appraise the stimulus as non-serious or unimportant, putting us into a playful frame of mind at least momentarily
emotional response
- comprises an emotional response that is elicited by these appraisals
- referred to as ‘mirth’
–> “pleasurable feeling, joy, happiness”
vocal-behavioural expression of laughter
Mirth is expressed as smiling/laughing, communicating to others we are in a playful state
types of humour
- jokes
- spontaneous conversational humour
- unintentional humour
jokes
- Short, amusing stories consists of a setup and a punch line
- The setup, which includes all but the last sentence, creates expectations in the listener about how the situation should be interpreted
- The punch line suddenly shifts the meaning in an unexpected and playful way, thus creating the perception of non-serious incongruity that is necessary for humour to occur
spontaneous conversational humour
- 72% of daily laughter occurs spontaneously during social interactions either in response to funny comments or amusing anecdotes
- More context-dependent than joke-telling, and is therefore often not as funny when recounted afterwards
–> “You had to be there”
different forms of spontaneous conversational humour
- anecdotes
–> relating an amusing story about oneself or someone else - wordplay
–> creating puns, witty responses, or wisecracks that play on the meaning of words - irony
–> a statement in which the literal meaning is different from the intended meaning
unintentional humour
- physical or linguistic
- accidental physical humour includes minor mishaps and blunders.
–> funny when they occur in a surprising and incongruous manner
–> the person experiencing them is not seriously hurt or badly embarrassed - accidental linguistic humour arises from misspellings, mispronunciations, errors in logic, and the kinds of speaker confusions called Freudian slips, malapropisms, and spoonerisms
–> e.g. calling ‘miss’ ‘mum’ by accident
4 theoretical explanations of humour
- evolutionary theory
- superiority theory
- incongruity theory
- relief theory
socio emotional functions of humour - evolutionary theory
- contributes to survival
- a witty sense of humour, like linguistic skills and creativity, is an indicator of superior intelligence, enhancing one’s ability to compete successfully for resources
- laughter is a form of “vocal grooming” which, like physical grooming in primates, facilitates social bonding
- laughter is a “false alarm,” –> signaling to others that a stimulus or event is unimportant and non-serious
evolutionary theory
- Humour and laughter are a universal aspect of human experience
- Developmentally, laughter is one of the first social vocalizations (after crying) emitted by human infants
–> even children born deaf and blind can laugh without ever having perceived the laughter of others - Laughter/humour has also been observed in chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and gorillas
- Laughter appears to have originated in social play and to be derived from primate play signals
- Humans have now adapted to mental play with words = humour
superiority theory
- laughter originates in malice
–> we laugh at what is ridiculous in other people
–> feeling delight instead of pain when we see even our friends in misfortune - humour is thought to result from a sense of superiority we feel from the belittling of another person or of one’s own past blunders or foolishness
- humour is ‘playful aggression’
–> generations of men responded to their sudden victories in violent encounters with roars of triumph
–> this early origin of laughter evolved into our modern-day humour, people can now use language to make fun of those inferior
schadenfreude (Harm-joy)
the joy of another’s misfortune
evaluation of superiority theory
- strength:
–> studies show that aggressive elements in jokes and the perception of pain in others contribute to the funniness of the humour - limitations:
–> little evidence that all humour involves some form of aggression and that aggressive people enjoy all types of humour more than non-aggressive people
–> animal studies suggest facial displays associated with humour (playful face, smiling, laughter) only occur in the context of friendly social/play activities, not aggression
–> essentially unfalsifiable, Gruner defined playful aggression so broadly that he claims the theory seems to account for all humour
incongruity theory
- Suggest that the perception of incongruity is the crucial determinant of whether or not something is humorous
–> incongruous = surprising, peculiar, unusual, or different from what we normally expect - However, for incongruity to be funny, it must also be resolved or “make sense” in some way
–> resolution of incongruity in a joke is what makes it possible for us to get the joke - We go back and search for an ambiguity in the joke setup that can be interpreted in a different way so that the punch line makes sense
–> humour comprehension can be seen as a sort of problem-solving task
brief summary of incongruity theory
- we predict the outcome of the set up
- if it as predicted, it isn’t funny
- if the punchline is surprising, we either:
–> understand the rule that makes the incongruity funny and therefore laugh
–> don’t understand the rule, don’t understand the incongruity and so we are puzzled
Shultz & Horibe, 1974 - method
- presented different versions of several jokes to kids aged 6, 8, 10 and 12
- original:
–> “Waiter, what’s this? … That’s bean soup, ma’am…
I’m not interested in what it’s been… I’m asking what it is now” - resolution removed:
–> “Waiter, what’s this? … That’s tomato soup, ma’am…
I’m not interested in what it’s been… I’m asking what it is now” - incongruity removed:
–> “Waiter, what’s this? … That’s bean soup, ma’am…
That’s what I thought, but I wasn’t sure” - kids then asked to rate how funny they are
Shultz & Horibe, 1974 - results
- By age 8, children found the original versions of the jokes funnier than resolution removed or incongruity removed versions
- When children are old enough to engage in cognitive processes and problem solving, they appreciated resolvable incongruity and found jokes with these elements funnier
- Suggesting that resolution of incongruity is needed for something to be perceived as funny