Exam 2 Flashcards

Study the concepts and make sure that you understand

1
Q

What is the difference between ideographic writing and a true writing system?

A

Definition: An ideographic system uses symbols or characters to represent ideas or concepts directly, rather than specific words, sounds, or syllables.

One symbol can represent an abstract idea (e.g., ☀️ could mean “sun,” “day,” or “light”).
May not correspond directly to the phonetic structure of a spoken language.
Can be difficult to learn because there is often a large set of symbols with specific meanings.
———————————————————
2. True Writing System
Definition: A true writing system represents the spoken language by encoding phonetic elements (like sounds or syllables) or words. It allows for the full expression of the language as it is spoken.
Examples: The English alphabet, Arabic script, and the Japanese Kana system.
Characteristics:
Alphabetic systems (e.g., English) use letters to represent individual sounds (phonemes).
Syllabaries (e.g., Japanese Kana) represent whole syllables.
Logographic components (e.g., in Chinese) represent whole words but still function systematically within the language.
———————————————————
What are the key differences:
Phonetic vs conceptual:A true writing system encodes the sounds or words of a language, whereas ideographic writing focuses on concepts or ideas directly.
Language Dependency:A true writing system aligns with the structure of a particular spoken language. In contrast, ideographic systems can, in theory, be interpreted across languages without depending on phonetics (though some languages, like Chinese, blur this distinction).
Expressiveness: A true writing system can represent complex grammatical structures and abstract ideas as spoken, while ideographic systems are limited in this respect.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

2) What are the differences among logographic, syllabic, and alphabetic writing
systems?

A
  1. Logographic Writing System
    Definition: Each symbol represents a word, morpheme, or idea rather than individual sounds.
    Examples: Chinese characters, ancient Egyptian hieroglyphs (in part), and Mayan glyphs.
    Characteristics:
    One symbol = one meaning (e.g., 木 = “tree” in Chinese).
    A single character may carry multiple meanings depending on context.
    Often requires thousands of symbols to fully express a language.
    Advantages: Allows for concise visual representation; can transcend spoken language differences.
    Challenges: Very large number of symbols to learn and remember
  2. syllabic writing systems:
    Definition: Each symbol corresponds to a syllable, a basic unit of sound typically consisting of a consonant-vowel pair or just a vowel.
    Examples: Japanese Kana (Hiragana and Katakana), Cherokee script.
    Characteristics:
    One symbol = one syllable (e.g., か in Hiragana represents the sound “ka”).
    Fewer symbols than logographic systems, but still more than alphabetic systems.
    Advantages: Easier to learn than logographic systems due to fewer symbols.
    Challenges: Less compact than alphabetic systems because each syllable needs its own symbol.

Alphabetic Writing System
Definition: Each symbol represents an individual sound (phoneme), like a consonant or a vowel.
Examples: English, Greek, Arabic, Russian.
Characteristics:
One symbol = one sound (e.g., the letter “b” represents the /b/ sound in English).
The smallest set of symbols among the three types, typically 20-30 letters.
Advantages: Efficient and flexible for representing a variety of sounds and words.
Challenges: Requires knowledge of spelling conventions and phonetic rules, which can vary by language (e.g., English has irregular spellings).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

3) What is a transparent alphabetic writing system? Is English transparent in this
sense?

A

What is a Transparent Alphabetic Writing System?
A transparent alphabetic writing system is one in which the grapheme-to-phoneme correspondence (how letters represent sounds) is consistent and predictable.
In a transparent system, each letter (or combination of letters) corresponds reliably to the same sound(s), and there are few irregularities or exceptions.
This makes the process of decoding (reading aloud) and encoding (writing) straightforward, especially for learners.
Examples of Transparent Alphabetic Systems:

Spanish:
Each letter consistently represents the same sound (e.g., “a” is always pronounced /a/).
Finnish:
Almost one-to-one correspondence between letters and sounds, making it one of the most transparent languages.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Is English transparent in this
sense?

A

Is English Transparent?
No, English is not considered a transparent alphabetic writing system. It is classified as an opaque (or deep) orthography because of its inconsistent spelling rules and irregular pronunciation.

Reasons Why English is Opaque:

Inconsistent Grapheme-to-Phoneme Correspondence:
The same letter or letter combination can represent different sounds.
Example: “ough” in though, through, tough, and thought.
Multiple Spellings for the Same Sound:
Example: The /f/ sound is represented differently in fun, photo, and enough.
Historical Influences:
English has borrowed words from many languages (Latin, French, Greek, etc.), which has introduced a variety of spelling patterns.
Silent Letters:
Example: The “k” in knight or the “b” in comb are not pronounced.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

What are the distinctions among word, morpheme, and phoneme levels of spelling
representation?

A

Word Level Representation
Definition: This level deals with whole words as the basic unit of meaning. Spelling representation at this level focuses on the correct arrangement of letters to form complete words.
Examples:
“cat”, “house”, “running”
Characteristics:
Entire words are stored in the mental lexicon and recognized as wholes.
Useful for languages with many irregular spellings (e.g., English words like “colonel” or “island”).
Sight words (words memorized as whole units) play a crucial role in early reading development.

Definition: A morpheme is the smallest meaningful unit in a language. This level of representation considers both roots and affixes (prefixes, suffixes, etc.) in spelling.
Examples:
In the word “unhappiness,” there are three morphemes:
“un-“ (prefix, meaning “not”)
“happy” (root)
“-ness” (suffix, meaning a state or quality)
Characteristics:
Morphological spelling patterns reflect word structure, even if pronunciation changes.
Example: The past tense “-ed” is pronounced differently in “wanted” (/ɪd/) vs. “walked” (/t/), but spelled the same.
Important for understanding inflection (changing word form for tense, number, etc.) and derivation (forming new words by adding affixes).

Phoneme Level Representation
Definition: A phoneme is the smallest unit of sound that can distinguish meaning in a language. Phoneme-level spelling reflects how individual sounds (phonemes) map onto letters or letter combinations (graphemes).
Examples:
In “cat”:
/k/ = “c”
/æ/ = “a”
/t/ = “t”
Characteristics:
Phonemic spelling tries to represent each sound systematically (e.g., “bat” vs. “pat” differ by one phoneme).
Transparent alphabetic languages (like Spanish) have a closer match between phonemes and graphemes than opaque languages (like English).
In English, inconsistent phoneme-grapheme mappings create spelling challenges (e.g., “ough” in “though” vs. “rough”).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

How does the tendency to preserve morphemic information in spelling contribute
to the non-transparency of English spelling?

A

Morphemic preservation means that spelling stays consistent across related words, even if the pronunciation changes. The goal is to retain the recognition of common roots, prefixes, and suffixes across word forms, which helps convey meaning but creates discrepancies between spelling and pronunciation.

How This Affects Transparency
Lack of 1:1 correspondence between sounds and letters:
The preservation of morphemes makes it difficult to predict pronunciation just by looking at the spelling.
Increased cognitive load for learners:
English learners must memorize morphemic patterns in addition to mastering the phonetic rules, which adds complexity.
Opaque orthography:
Because spelling prioritizes morphological consistency over phonetic accuracy, English has a deeper (more opaque) orthography than languages like Spanish or Finnish, which focus on phoneme-to-grapheme correspondence.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What is the difference between a phoneme and a speech sound?

A
  1. Phoneme
    Definition:
    A phoneme is the smallest unit of sound that can change the meaning of a word in a particular language. It is an abstract category that represents a group of similar sounds.
    Example:
    In English, /p/ and /b/ are two distinct phonemes. They differ only slightly in articulation (one is voiceless, the other voiced), but changing /p/ to /b/ changes the word:
    “pat” → “bat”
    Characteristics:
    Abstract: Phonemes exist in the mind as linguistic units, not as physical sounds.
    Language-specific: Phonemes are specific to a given language; for example, some languages distinguish between sounds that others treat as identical.
    Minimal pairs: Phonemes are often identified by finding minimal pairs—pairs of words where only one sound is different, resulting in a change in meaning (e.g., “bit” vs. “bat”).
  2. Speech Sound (Phone)
    Definition:
    A speech sound (or phone) is the physical realization of a phoneme. It refers to the actual sound produced by the vocal tract when speaking.
    Example:
    The English phoneme /p/ has two possible speech sounds (phones):
    [pʰ] (aspirated “p”) in “pat.”
    [p] (unaspirated “p”) in “spat.”
    Characteristics:
    Concrete: A phone is a measurable, physical sound that can be captured and analyzed acoustically.
    Allophony: Different speech sounds (called allophones) can represent the same phoneme, depending on the context. For example, both [pʰ] and [p] are allophones of /p/ in English.
    Language-independent: Speech sounds exist across languages, but how they function and are categorized differs by language.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

what are the key differences between each other?

A

Phoneme
Definition: Abstract unit of sound with meaning
Example: /p/ vs. /b/ (changes meaning)
Abstract or Concrete: Abstract (mental representation)
Function: Differentiates meaning in a language
Allophony: Includes multiple allophones
Language-specific? Yes

Speech Sound (Phone)
Definition: Physical production of a sound
Example: [pʰ] vs. [p] (does not change meaning)
Abstract or Concrete: Concrete (physical realization)
Function: May or may not affect meaning
Allophony: A specific allophone or variant
Language-specific? No (phones can exist in multiple languages)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

What is articulatory?

A

Having to do with the peripheral processes for articulating words – for
example, the processes for positioning and moving the lips and tongue while
speaking. A selective articulatory deficit would be one in which a patient’s
knowledge of the sounds of words was intact, but she produced mispronunciations because of impairment in controlling the parts of the vocal tract used to produce
words.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

What is auditorially?

A

In the form of sounds. Auditorially presented words are words that are
presented in spoken (as opposed to written) form

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

What is cerebral vascular accident (CVA)?

A

In incident in which obstruction or rupture of a
blood vessel in the brain cuts off or reduces the blood supply to an area of the
brain, causing temporary dysfunction or – if the reduction in blood supply is
sufficiently severe and prolonged, death of the affected brain tissue

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

what is dissociation?

A

Pattern of results in which one task or cognitive ability (e.g., ability to
recognize faces) shows impairment, while another task or ability (e.g., ability to
recognize objects) is intact, or at least much less impaired

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What is excision?

A

cutting out, as in tumor excision

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

what is a lesion?

A

eneral term for tissue damage. A brain lesion is a site of damage in the brain

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What is a lexical? What is a lexicon?

A

Having to do with words.

Literally, a list of the words in a language. In cognitive neuropsychology
usually refers to a ‘mental dictionary’ that stores certain forms of knowledge
about words. For example, an orthographic lexicon stores information about the
written forms (orthography) of words, and a phonological lexicon stores
information about spoken word form (phonology)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

What is locus?

A

A location or place. The locus of brain damage is the place in the
brain that is damaged.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

What is a motor plan?

A

A brain representation of the movements required to carry out some action,
such as throwing a ball or articulating a word.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

What is a peripheral?

A

In cognitive science, having to do with the initial stages in processing
sensory stimuli, or the final stages in producing a movement or other response.
Contrasted with central. For example, in spoken production of a word, processes
that select a word form on the basis of a to-be-expressed meaning are central,
whereas processes that control the articulatory muscles are peripheral.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

What is a Positron emission tomography (PET)?

A

A functional imaging technique for determining
what parts of the brain are active when a person is performing a task

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

What is premorbid.?

A

Prior to the onset of an illness or injury. For example, a stroke patient’s
premorbid state is his/her condition prior to the stroke

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

What is semantic?

A

Having to do with meaning.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

What is syntax

A

The aspect of language that has to do with how words or morphemes are
combined into higher-level units such as sentences. For example, syntactic
knowledge tells you that in the sentence The cat chased the dog it is the cat that is
doing the chasing.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

What is the distinction between form and meaning as applied to lexical knowledge?

A
  1. Form

Definition: Refers to the physical structure or representation of a word—how it is spoken, written, or constructed grammatically.
Components of Form:
Phonology: How the word sounds when spoken.
Orthography: How the word is written or spelled.
Morphology: The structure of the word (roots, prefixes, suffixes).
Example:
Word: “cats”
Form:
Pronunciation: /kæts/
Spelling: C-A-T-S
Morphology: Root = “cat,” Suffix = “-s” (plural marker)
Relevance to Lexical Knowledge: Knowing the form helps with recognizing, pronouncing, and spelling words correctly.

Definition: Refers to the semantic or conceptual aspect of a word—what it refers to, describes, or conveys.
Components of Meaning:
Denotation: The literal or dictionary meaning (e.g., “cat” = a small domesticated animal).
Connotation: The emotional or cultural associations of a word (e.g., “home” suggests warmth and comfort beyond its literal meaning).
Polysemy: Words with multiple meanings depending on context (e.g., “run” as in “running fast” vs. “running a business”).
Example:
Word: “cats”
Meaning: Refers to more than one domesticated feline animal (plural form of “cat”).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

What reasons do Rapp & Caramazza provide for thinking that knowledge of form is
separate from knowledge of meaning?

A

Back (Answer):
Neuropsychological Evidence: Double Dissociation
Double dissociation between form (phonology/orthography) and meaning (semantics) in brain-damaged patients:
Some patients can spell or pronounce words correctly but cannot understand their meanings.
Others know the meaning of words but cannot produce the correct form (e.g., spelling or pronunciation errors).
Conclusion: Form and meaning operate independently in the brain.
Lexical Access without Semantic Access
Patients can read aloud or spell words correctly without knowing what those words mean.
They can also produce non-words correctly (e.g., pronounce or spell “blark”) following rules of phonology or orthography.
Conclusion: Phonological/orthographic knowledge can be accessed without needing semantic knowledge.
Modality-Specific Deficits
Separate impairments in spoken and written word forms:
Some patients struggle with spoken words (phonology) but are able to read and write correctly (orthography).
Others struggle with writing but retain spoken language abilities.
Conclusion: Phonological and orthographic systems are distinct from each other and from the semantic system.
Implications for Cognitive Models of Language Processing
Supports modular models of language:
Phonological and orthographic processing are separate from semantic processing.
The brain processes these forms in parallel and can continue using one system even if the other is impaired.
Summary of Key Insight:
Form (phonology and orthography) and meaning (semantics) are processed by independent neural systems.
This separation explains why patients with brain damage may have intact form knowledge but impaired meaning knowledge, or vice versa.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

What hypotheses do Rapp & Caramazza consider regarding the relationship between
orthographic and phonological knowledge? Which hypothesis do they favor? What
supporting evidence do they present?

A

Hypotheses Considered:
Shared Storage Hypothesis (Integrated Representation):
This hypothesis suggests that orthographic and phonological knowledge are stored together in a shared, unified system.
Prediction: Damage to one form of representation (e.g., phonological) would lead to impairments in the other form (e.g., orthographic).
Example: If phonological representations are impaired, spelling ability would also be affected.
Independent Storage Hypothesis (Modular Representation):
This hypothesis proposes that orthographic knowledge (written form) and phonological knowledge (spoken form) are stored in separate, independent systems.
Prediction: Damage to one system (e.g., phonological) does not necessarily impair the other system (e.g., orthographic), meaning individuals can retain spelling knowledge even if they struggle with pronunciation or vice versa.
Interactive Storage Hypothesis:
This hypothesis proposes that while orthographic and phonological knowledge are separately stored, there is a high degree of interaction between them during use.
Prediction: While orthographic and phonological systems are independent, frequent interaction between them means damage to one might still impair performance in tasks involving both (e.g., reading aloud).
Which Hypothesis Do Rapp & Caramazza Favor?
Rapp & Caramazza favor the Independent Storage Hypothesis.
They argue that orthographic and phonological knowledge are stored independently in distinct cognitive systems.
Their neuropsychological research shows that orthographic and phonological impairments can occur independently in brain-damaged patients, supporting the idea that these systems are modular and not stored together.
Supporting Evidence Presented by Rapp & Caramazza:
Double Dissociations in Patients:
They present cases of brain-damaged patients who show:
Intact phonological knowledge (e.g., they can pronounce words correctly) but impaired orthographic knowledge (e.g., spelling errors).
Intact orthographic knowledge (e.g., they can spell correctly) but impaired phonological knowledge (e.g., pronunciation difficulties).
Conclusion: These dissociations indicate that orthographic and phonological systems operate independently.
Specific Case Studies:
Some patients are able to write words correctly but mispronounce them when reading aloud, suggesting separate orthographic and phonological representations.
Other patients can pronounce words but struggle to produce the correct spelling, further supporting independent systems.
Task-Specific Deficits:
Rapp & Caramazza highlight that some tasks rely exclusively on orthographic or phonological systems:
Silent reading primarily involves orthographic processing, while reading aloud requires both phonological and orthographic systems.
Patients who perform well in writing tasks but poorly in phonological tasks offer clear evidence of independent representations.
Theoretical Implications:
Their findings support a modular model of language processing, where orthography and phonology are independent systems with distinct neural bases, though they can interact when needed (e.g., during reading or speaking).
Conclusion:
Rapp & Caramazza favor the Independent Storage Hypothesis.
They argue that orthographic and phonological knowledge are stored separately in the brain.
The evidence of double dissociation in patients (e.g., intact spelling with impaired pronunciation and vice versa) supports the idea of modularity in language processing systems.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

What is the definition of the logos?

A

The earliest true writing systems are based, then, on the one-word-one-
symbol principle. Such writing systems are called “logographic” (from
the Greek word logos meaning word), and individual symbols are known
as “logograms.”

27
Q

What is Logographic Writing:

A

Logographic Writing:
Definition: A system where each symbol (called a logogram) corresponds to a specific word in the language, rather than just representing a general concept.
Examples:
Chinese characters and Japanese Kanji are modern examples of logographic systems.
Historical Examples:
Sumerian cuneiform and Egyptian hieroglyphics started as picture-based representations but evolved to become more abstract and word-based.

28
Q

What is idelography?

A

Ideographic Writing:
Definition: Writing in which symbols represent concepts or ideas rather than specific words.
Examples:
A circle could represent the sun, but also light, heat, day, or a sun god.
Limitation: Messages in ideographic writing are open to multiple interpretations since symbols can refer to various ideas.
Modern Example: Some symbols like emojis can function ideographically, though they are not considered full writing systems.

29
Q

What is Key Transition in Writing Systems:

A

Key Transition in Writing Systems:
Over time, logograms became more abstract and were used not only for their original meanings but also to represent similar-sounding words.
Example: A logogram for “sun” could also represent the word “son” if both words sound the same.
Borrowed Example: The combination of two logograms for “bee” and “leaf” could represent the word “belief.”

30
Q

What is Historical Development of Writing?

A

Writing systems independently evolved in multiple places (like Mesopotamia and Egypt) through similar steps:
Picture writing (representing objects).
Ideographic writing (representing ideas or concepts).
Logographic writing (representing specific words in language).
Phonetic use of logograms (symbols used for word-sounds, regardless of meaning).

31
Q

What is the difference between a transparent and opaque writing system? Provide examples.

A

Transparent writing system: The spelling of each word clearly matches its pronunciation.
Examples: Finnish, Italian, and Latvian.
Transparent English words: dog, ship, pistol – pronounced as expected based on their spelling.
Opaque writing system: Some words deviate from standard spelling rules, making their pronunciation unpredictable.
Examples of irregular English words: women, yacht, knight, debt, colonel, island.

32
Q

How did English spelling change after 1500 AD, and why did irregular spellings emerge?

A

ck (Answer):

Before 1500 AD: English words were spelled phonetically, based on pronunciation, which varied by region and dialect.
Introduction of irregular spellings:
Scribes disliked how words with repeated up-down strokes (like “wimin,” “munk,” “wunder”) looked in cursive writing.
To make rapid writing easier, scribes changed the spelling to “women,” “monk,” and “wonder.”

33
Q

How did false etymologies influence English spelling?

A

Reformers sometimes introduced incorrect letters based on mistaken origins.
Examples:
scissors and scythe: Based on the mistaken belief that they derived from Latin “scindere” (to cleave).
anchor: Linked incorrectly to the Greek word “anchorite.”
Other false etymology changes:
s in island: Formerly spelled iland (the “s” was never pronounced).
h in hour: Added based on a historical link to oure, but it was never pronounced in English.

34
Q

When and how was English spelling standardized?

A

1650: English spelling was mostly standardized.
1755: Dr. Samuel Johnson’s Dictionary of the English Language helped finalize standard spelling.
1770: The concept of a spelling error emerged; prior to that, if a word’s spelling sounded right, it was considered correc

35
Q

What caused irregularities in English spellings over time?

A

Pronunciation changes over centuries were not reflected in spelling.
Example: “knave” and “knife” were pronounced with a /k/ sound in the 17th century.
Would and should were pronounced with the /l/ sound.
“Gh” (as in “light” and “right”) originally had a /ch/ sound, similar to the Scottish “loch.”
As pronunciation changed, fossilized spellings remained in use, leading to spelling-sound mismatches.

36
Q

What caused irregularities in English spellings over time?

A

Pronunciation changes over centuries were not reflected in spelling.
Example: “knave” and “knife” were pronounced with a /k/ sound in the 17th century.
Would and should were pronounced with the /l/ sound.
“Gh” (as in “light” and “right”) originally had a /ch/ sound, similar to the Scottish “loch.”
As pronunciation changed, fossilized spellings remained in use, leading to spelling-sound mismatches.

37
Q

What is a morpheme, and what are the types of morphemes?

A

orpheme: The smallest meaningful unit of language.
Types of Morphemes:
Free morphemes: Can stand alone as words (e.g., trust).
Bound morphemes: Cannot stand alone and modify the root word (e.g., dis-, -ful, -ing, -ed).
Examples:

trustful: Two morphemes – trust (root) + -ful (bound morpheme).
distrustfully: Four morphemes – dis-, trust, -ful, -ly.

38
Q

What is a phoneme, and how does it relate to spelling?

A

Phoneme: The smallest unit of sound in a language that can distinguish meaning.
Example: The word “trust” contains five letters and five phonemes, each represented by a letter.
Some words contain fewer phonemes than letters.
Example: “Cheat” has five letters but only three phonemes:
ch, ea, t.

39
Q

Flashcard 3: Homophones and the Influence of Logographic Spelling

A

Back (Answer):

Although English uses the alphabetic system, it retains logographic elements:
Examples of logographs: &, $, £.
Homophones are words that sound the same but are spelled differently to reflect meaning:
Two vs. too
Hymn vs. him
Sign vs. sine
Why it matters:

If English spelling were reformed to only allow one spelling per phoneme, the visual distinction between homophones would be lost.

40
Q

Why is English spelling considered irregular, and how does it represent multiple linguistic levels?

A

ack (Answer):

Spelling reflects multiple levels:
Sentence level: Capital letters mark the beginning, and periods mark the end.
Clause and phrase level: Commas and semi-colons are used to mark boundaries.
Word level: Spaces separate words.
Irregular spellings:

English allows more than one way to represent the same phoneme (e.g., been vs. bean).
Some words with irregular spellings derive from historical and logographic influences (e.g., sword, hymn, damn, sign).

41
Q

What insight did Chomsky and Halle (1968) provide about English spelling?

A

n The Sound Pattern of English, Chomsky and Halle argued that English spelling reflects deep linguistic structures:
Spelling often preserves the morphemic structure of words, even when pronunciation changes.
Example: “sign” retains its spelling even though the /g/ is silent.
Conclusion: English spelling maintains both phonological and morphemic information, making it more complex but more informative.

42
Q

What is the trade-off between a transparent alphabetic system and English orthography?

A

Transparent writing systems: Each sound corresponds to one spelling (e.g., Finnish, Italian).
English orthography: Balances between phonological transparency and the retention of logographic principles:
Example: Retaining distinct spellings for homophones (e.g., too vs. two) helps convey meaning.
This trade-off results in irregular spellings but preserves meaningful distinctions in writing.

43
Q

Why do words like “sign” and “bomb” retain silent letters?

A

Silent letters in words like “sign” and “bomb” preserve the morphemic relationship with related words:
“Sign” → “signature”
“Bomb” → “bombard”
Chomsky and Halle’s Insight: Removing silent letters would obscure the morphological connection between these related words, making the shared meaning less obvious.

44
Q

What tension exists between phonemic transparency and morphemic preservation in English spelling?

A

Phonemic transparency: Spelling directly reflects pronunciation (e.g., ROPES → ROPEZ to indicate the plural /z/ sound).
Morphemic preservation: Spelling preserves the morphemic structure even if pronunciation changes.
Example: The plural -s is written the same way in ropes, robes, and roses, despite different pronunciations (/s/, /z/, /ɪz/).

45
Q

Why do we spell flapped, soared, and glided with “-ed” instead of phonetic forms like FLAPT or SOARD?

A

Why do we spell flapped, soared, and glided with “-ed” instead of phonetic forms like FLAPT or SOARD?

Back (Answer):

Morphemic consistency: The past tense is always represented by -ed, even though pronunciation varies:
Flapped = /t/ sound
Soared = /d/ sound
Glided = /ɪd/ sound
A phonetic spelling (e.g., FLAPT, SOARD) would lose the morphological connection to the past tense morpheme -ed.

46
Q

How does English spelling preserve plural morphemes despite varying pronunciations?

A

The plural morpheme is consistently written as -s in words like ropes, robes, and roses, even though the pronunciation differs:
Ropes = /s/ sound
Robes = /z/ sound
Roses = /ɪz/ sound
This consistency helps preserve the morphemic identity of the plural marker -s.

47
Q

What were the results of Baker’s (1980) experiment on spelling reform?

A

articipants were asked to spell words as either linguists or spelling reformers:
As linguists: They preferred phonetically transparent spellings like ROPEZ and ROSIZ.
As spelling reformers: They opted to preserve morphemic identity by writing ropes, robes, roses despite pronunciation differences.
Insight: There’s a trade-off between phonetic accuracy and morphological consistency in English spelling.

48
Q

Why doesn’t English use a syllabic writing system, like Japanese Kana?

A

Reason: English has too many distinct syllables to represent each one with a single symbol.
Example: Words like “Bar-B-Q” are rare exceptions where English uses syllabic representation.
Instead, English uses an alphabetic system that reflects individual phonemes rather than entire syllables.

49
Q

What is the relationship between phonemes and graphemes in English spelling?

A

Phoneme: The smallest sound unit that distinguishes meaning (e.g., /p/ in “pit” and /p/ in “spit”).
Grapheme: A letter or group of letters that represents a phoneme in written form.
Examples:
ch in “chip” = one phoneme (/ʧ/)
th in “bath” = one phoneme (/θ/)
oo in “moon” = one phoneme (/u:/)
Phonemic Transparency: When each phoneme corresponds directly to a grapheme (e.g., “dog”).

50
Q

Why is English spelling considered irregular and complex?

A

nconsistent rules: Some vowel sounds can be spelled multiple ways (e.g., “heat,” “street,” “complete”).
Inconsistent letter strings: The same letters can have different pronunciations in different words (e.g., “mint” vs. “pint”).
Final silent “e” rule: Changes the pronunciation of vowels (e.g., “bit” vs. “bite”).
These irregularities make English difficult for learners, although experienced readers can navigate them with ease.

51
Q

What are the three levels of representation in English spelling?

A

Word Level:
Use of logographs: &, $, %
Homophones are distinguished by different spellings:
be vs. bee, plain vs. plane
Related words share visual clues (e.g., “sign” and “signature”).
Morpheme Level:
Spelling preserves morphemic consistency, even if pronunciation changes:
Ropes, robes, roses (plural morpheme -s with varied pronunciation)
Flapped, soared, glided (past tense morpheme -ed with different sounds)
Phoneme Level:
Transparent spellings: Each letter corresponds to a phoneme (e.g., “bit,” “dull”).
However, English also has opaque spellings (e.g., “sign”).

52
Q

How does English spelling differentiate homophones?

A

ront (Question):
How does English spelling differentiate homophones?

Back (Answer):

Homophones: Words that sound the same but have different meanings.
Examples:
plain vs. plane
be vs. bee
rain, rein, reign
English spelling helps retain meaning visually by using different spellings for homophones, even if they sound identical.

53
Q

How does English spelling maintain morphemic consistency despite changes in pronunciation?

A

Back (Answer):

Plural morpheme -s:
Ropes = /s/
Robes = /z/
Roses = /ɪz/
The spelling -s stays the same across words to maintain consistency.
Past tense morpheme -ed:
Flapped = /t/
Soared = /d/
Glided = /ɪd/
The spelling -ed stays constant even though the pronunciation varies.

54
Q

What is the alphabetic principle, and how does it apply to English?

A

ack (Answer):

Alphabetic principle: Each letter (or group of letters) corresponds to a phoneme.
Example: dog = /d/ /ɒ/ /g/
In English, grapheme-to-phoneme correspondence is not always consistent:
Example: The “oo” in “moon” vs. “book.”
English orthography combines phoneme representation with morphemic preservation.

55
Q

What did Chomsky and Halle observe about English spelling?

A

English spelling preserves morphemic relationships even at the expense of phonetic accuracy.
Example: “sign” retains the “g” to indicate its relationship to “signature”.
The alphabetic system reflects not only phonemes but also morphemic and word-level information, making English spelling complex but informative.

56
Q

What does lexical knowledge refer to in cognitive neuroscience?

A

Lexical knowledge is our knowledge of words, including their meaning, phonology, orthography, and syntax.
According to Rapp and Caramazza, this knowledge is internally complex and organized into distinct components.

57
Q

How is the independent representation of meaning and form shown in lexical processing?

A

Evidence from anomia (difficulty retrieving word forms while knowing meaning) shows that meaning and form can be processed independently.
Example: E.S.T. (a patient) could describe a “snowman” but could not produce its name.
Suggests that meaning and form of words have separate representations in the brain.

58
Q

What does spared knowledge of form indicate in cases of impaired comprehension?

A

Cases where patients can read or spell words without understanding them imply a separation between form (spelling, pronunciation) and meaning.
Example: Patient J.J. could spell “type” correctly but gave incorrect definitions, showing form knowledge without meaning comprehension.

59
Q

What evidence supports the independent access to orthographic and phonological forms?

A

Back (Answer):

Some patients show better written than spoken naming ability, suggesting orthography and phonology are separately accessed.
Example: Patients like P.W. could write “S-O-C-K” (sock) correctly but said “skirt” aloud, indicating orthographic independence from phonology.

60
Q

What are category-specific semantic deficits?

A

Patients show selective impairment for certain categories, like animate vs. inanimate concepts.
Example: J.B.R. could describe “briefcase” (inanimate) but struggled with “ostrich” (animate).
Indicates potential distinct neural systems for processing different categories of words.

61
Q

How does complexity and familiarity affect category-specific semantic deficits?

A

Some researchers suggest that complexity and familiarity influence semantic deficits.
However, studies like Sartori et al. (1993) show category-specific deficits even when complexity and familiarity are controlled.

62
Q

What is the sensory-specific semantic hypothesis?

A

Proposes that animate concepts rely on visual features, while inanimate concepts rely on functional properties.
Implication: Damage to visual semantics would impair animate concepts more than inanimate ones.

63
Q

What do morphological errors reveal about lexical representation?

A

Patients with morphological errors (e.g., producing “talking” for “talked”) suggest morpheme-based processing.
S.J.D. made errors like “endangered” to “endangerous,” indicating a miscombination of morpheme units in lexical representation.

64
Q
A