Emotions & Temperament Flashcards

1
Q

Emotion vs. affect

A
  • an internal, affective response about something in environment
  • general positive or negative feeling
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

2 broad categories of emotion

A
  1. primary/basic: joy, sadness, anger, fear, disgust, interest, surprise
  2. secondary/self-conscious: requires development of sense of self and other; can be evaluative or non-evaluative

  • evaluative: shame, pride, guilt (involves societal standards)
  • non-evaluative: embarassment, envy
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Discrete emotions theory

A
  • different emotions reflect discrete systems that evolved as universal biological reactions to common challenges
  • each emotion has an expression, neural signature, physiological state
  • universally experienced and detected
  • emerge at particular times in infancy
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Emergent theory of emotions

A
  • emotions are the outcome of a process involving changes in body and cognition about what’s happening in environment
  • emotions are global early in infancy (e.g. positive/negative affect) then become more, but not completely, discrete
  • different people experience different emotions differently (e.g. crying due to sadness, happiness, anger)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

2 ways to measure emotion

A
  1. behavioral changes: facial expressions, vocalizations, approach/avoidance
  2. physiological changes: heart rate changes, sweating, brain responses
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Emotional expressions vs. display rules

A
  • emotional expressions: observable behavioral responses associated with emotions
  • display rules: how and when one should express emotions

  • babies/kids notoriously bad at display rules (though some differences observable in infancy)
  • display rules vary by culture, gender, and must be learned
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

When is the first cry-face seen?

A

from ~20 weeks gestation

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Evolution of happiness from 2-5 months

A
  • neonatal smile: due to internal sensations, mainly during REM sleep
  • smiling due to external stimuli (3-8 wks): due to touching, high-pitched voice, etc.
  • social smile (6-8 wks): typically to familiar people and one-to-one interactions (cross-cultural differences)
  • laughing (2-5 mos): during interesting/positive sensations and stimuli (also have cross-cultural differences)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

When and how can you elicit anger in infants?

A

2-4 mos: removing interesting objects/events, arm-holding, preventing them from reaching goals, contingency disruption paradigms (perhaps because requires means-ends reasoning)

  • newborns have general distress (e.g. hunger, pain, cold) when under/overstimulated
  • peaks between 18-24 mos as infants increasingly want control over environment
  • sadness elicited in similar situations, including when caregivers leave/become non-responsive
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

When do babies first show signs of fear?

A
  • by ~7 mos, same time as when they recognize (and focus on) fear expressions in others
  • fear bias: preferentially look to/fail to disengage from fearful faces (ERP brain responses selective to fear)

also when they begin to match vocal and facial emotion!

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Stranger vs. separation anxiety

fear

A

stranger anxiety: fear of strangers
* clearly in place by 7-8 mos, peaks at 8-12 mos, less by age 2
* individual differences (temperament, experience, situation) and cultural differences

separation anxiety: fear of caregiver leaving
* begins ~8 mos, declines by ~15 mos
* cross-cultural universal despite differences in child-rearing practices

  • stranger anxiety is greater outside home, if not in parents’ laps, and to males
  • little stranger anxiety among Efe people in Congo where group caregiving is common
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

How do babies experience surprise?

A

a reaction to the unexpected (~6 mos)
* not same as startle (present from birth)
* relatively infrequent (e.g. not clearly observable in violation of expectation experiments)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

How do babies experience disgust?

A
  • distaste for bitter tastes present at birth
  • babies generally don’t get disgusted by gross things
  • broader disgust emerges aroung age 4
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

How do babies experience secondary/self-conscious emotions?

A

begin by ~18-24 mos (require sense of self)
* shame/embarrassment: eyes lowered, head hung, hiding face
* guilt: reparations (positively-oriented compared to shame)
* pride after success (not until age 2)

individual and cross-cultural differences in tendency to experience these emotions (cultures also value them differently!)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Broken toy procedure

self-conscious emotions

A

trick 24-month-olds into thinking they broke experimenter’s favorite toy; respond with:
* shame (avoid experimenter, deny guilt) or
* guilt (fix toy, admit they broke it)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

When do babies begin distinguishing emotions?

different from understanding!

A

by 3 mos, babies distinguish between clearly different expressions based on habituation (e.g. teeth vs. no teeth)

17
Q

Social referencing

A

by 10-12 mos, babies use others’ emotional reactions to appraise a novel situation

  • negativity bias: negative signals are more likely to change babies’ behavior
  • younger babies need facial and, especially, vocal cues
  • more likely to retain messages with age
18
Q

Emotion regulation

A

strategies for adjusting our emotional states to a comfortable level of intensity

e.g. attention focusing/shifting, inhibiting thoughts/behaviors, planning

19
Q

Evidence for self-regulation in babies

but still need parents help!

A
  • young babies fall asleep if overwhelmed
  • 4 mos: look away from aversive stimuli
  • 4-6 mos: self-soothe via physical sensations (e.g. learn through sleep training)

toddlers generally not well-regulated (e.g. tantrums from communication difficulties) though highly culturally-variable

20
Q

Co-regulation

A

parents regulate babies’ emotions because they are unable to do it on their own by slowly increasing stimulation over first months (e.g. skin-to-skin holding, rocking, shushing, feedin)

  • touching premature babies in NICU has long-lasting benefits
  • US babies who were held/cuddled more had better emotional functioning as adults
21
Q

Consequence of failure to co-regulate

A

incorrect development of stress systems (e.g. can develop colic)

22
Q

Still-face paradigm

A
  • alternatingly, mother interacts normally with baby (2 mins) then adopts a neutral expression
  • infants show distress during “still-face” phases (e.g. attempts to get mother back or self-regulate, less attentive)
23
Q

Effect of depressed mothers on infants

A
  • flat affect, sleep issues, less attentive to surroundings and sad faces (suggests happy faces are novel)
  • negative interactions with mom and strangers (attachment issues)
  • more right frontal EEG asymmetry (withdrawal) and more cortisol
24
Q

Effect of anxious mothers on infants

A
  • more wary of strangers over time (if also inhibited)
  • less able to disengage from angry faces
25
Q

Temperament

A
  • stable individual differences in emotional reactivity and regulation
  • present from infancy and tied to biology but also influenced by environment

emotional reactivity: quickness and intensity of emotional arousal, attention, regulation

26
Q

6 dimensions used to measure temperament in babies

NY longitudinal study (Thomas & Chess) vs. Rothbart

A
  • Thomas & Chess: activity level, adaptability, intensity, regularity, approach/withdrawal, mood
  • Rothbart: activity level, attention span/persistence, fearful distress, irritable distress, positive affect, effortful control/inhibition

resulted in similar temperament range across cultures (with some proportional differences)

27
Q

3 types of babies according to temperament

Thomas & Chess

A
  1. easy (40%): happy, adaptable, regular routines, not under/over-sensitive
  2. slow to warm up (15%): negative, unadaptable, low activity and intensity, withdrawn
  3. difficult (10%): unhappy, unadaptable, irregular, intense reactions

Predicts functioning as adults: shyness in slow to warm up infants and high risk for adjustment problems in difficult infants

28
Q

Inhibited infants

Kagan, Fox, et al.

A

15% of 4-month-olds
* inherit a physiology that predisposes them to fear
* overstimulated/upset by novelty
* helped by warm, supportive parenting (without overprotection)

e.g. “fostering” rhesus studies: reactive babies with laid-back parents became less fearful

29
Q

Predictions for inhibited infants

Kagan, Fox, et al.

A
  • predispositions only retained 20-30% of time
  • tend to become shy with peer difficulties and 4-6x more likely to develop anxiety (a cascade!)

compared to uninhibited infants who tend to become sociable

30
Q

Physiological symptoms of inhibited infants

A

in response to novelty,
* greater amygdala reactivity
* higher heart rates
* higher cortisol
* greater pupil dilation
* higher blood pressure
* greater cooling of fingertips

31
Q

Baseline EEG activity to a neutral stimulus

inhibited vs. uninhibited infants

A
  • inhibited infants show greater activity on the right (right frontal asymmetry), associated with negative stimuli
  • uninhibited infants show greater activity on the left (left frontal asymmetry), associated with positive stimuli
32
Q

How stable is an infant’s temperament?

A

not very stable
* increase in attentional control with age (e.g. BI babies may learn to self-regulate by focusing on other stimuli)
* develop personality (temperament + environment), where environmental influences (e.g. parenting, culture) can either promote stability or change
* stability is greater later in life

33
Q

Goodness of fit

A

despite different temperaments, most children can flourish if placed in the right environment (and can suffer in wrong one), so must alter environment to best suit each child

e.g. children that inherit “difficult” genes from parents may induce harsher parenting

34
Q

Temperament of East Asian infants

compared to North American/European infants

A

calmer but more wary of strangers
* less active, irritable, vocal, and emotionally expressive
* more easily soothed by self and others
* more fearful

likely due to focus on gentle soothing of infants and close family ties vs. active stimulation and independence

35
Q

Which temperament is valued in East vs. West?

A

West
* inhibited is bad, independence/boldness is good
* shyness associated with negative outcomes and decreases over time

vs. East (opposite!)

differences diminishing over time as Western ideals take over (e.g. shyness now more associated with negative outcomes in China)

36
Q

How does self-concept develop?

A

sense of oneself as a physical body, separate from others
* observations that some parts of the world always stick with you as you go (e.g. self vs. attachment figure)
* experiencing your effect on the environment

37
Q

Evolution of self-concept

A
  • 2-4 mos: learn contingency with environment and display positive/negative emotion (e.g. mobile task)
  • 8 mos: show separation anxiety
  • 1 yr: joint attention and directing others’ attention
  • 2 yrs: recognize self in photographs and show self-conscious emotions

  • mobile task: show anger when contingency (e.g. ability to control mobile) is removed
  • young infants prefer those who imitate (interact contingently) them but distinguish those inividuals from themselves