Cultural Variations in Attachment - van Ijzendoorn Flashcards

1
Q

what are cultural variations

A

Cultural variations: differences between cultures
e.g. differences in attachment types between different cultural groupings.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

what is culture

A

The rules, customs, morals and ways of interacting that bind together members of a society or some other collection of people.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

what is subculture

A

A group within a country that, although it shares many of the dominant cultural characteristics of that country, may also have some special different characteristics.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

what is an individualist cultures

A

Importance is placed on the individual and achieving your own personal goals.
Being able to stand on your own two feet (independence) is valued.
E.g. USA, UK, Germany, Netherlands, Sweden.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

what is a collectivist culture

A

Importance is placed on the group as a whole.
e.g. Groups live and work together sharing tasks, belongings and child bearing
They aspire to be dependent on each other rather than function as individuals.
E.g. China, Israel, Japan.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg (1988) procedure

A

Procedure:
They conducted a meta-analysis of 32 studies from 8 countries that used the Strange Situation procedure, to assess similarities and differences in patterns of attachment types between (inter-cultural) and within (intra-cultural) cultures.
In total, the 32 studies examined 1,990 Strange Situation classifications (infants).
All studies comprised at least 35 mother-infant pairs with infants below 2 years of age.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

what is a meta-analysis

A

A META-ANALYSIS is a research method which collates and analyses data from many studies carried out by different researchers, gaining a broad overview of findings.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

What was the most common attachment type across the 8 countries studied?

A

secure

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Which 3 countries had the highest levels of insecure-resistant attachment types?

A

Japan (middle) - 27%
China (lowest) - 25%
Israel (highest) - 29%

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Which country had the highest level of insecure-avoidant types?

A

Germany - 35%

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Key Findings: van Ijzendoorn & Kroonenberg (1988)

A

Secure attachment was the most common classification in every country.

Insecure-avoidant attachment was the next most common in individualistic countries e.g. UK, USA.

Insecure-resistant attachment was more common in Israel, Japan and China (collectivist countries).

Intra-cultural (within cultures) differences were 1.5x greater than inter-cultural differences; Inter-cultural (between cultures) differences were small.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Conclusions: van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg

A

The global pattern across cultures appears to be similar to that found in the USA, in that secure attachment is the ‘norm’- it is the most common form of attachment in all countries

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

These cultural similarities therefore support the view that attachment is a innate. who’s theory does this support

A

This supports Bowlby’s monotropic theory.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

what does research suggest about some cultures

A

The researchers suggest that at least some cultural similarities might be explained by the effects of mass media (e.g. TV and books), which spread ideas about parenting norms (e.g. the need for sensitivity and responsiveness), so children throughout the world are exposed to similar influences.

This means that cultural similarities may not be due to innate biological influences, but are because of our increasingly global culture.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What could explain the variation found within cultures?

A

Some intra-cultural differences may be due to socio-economic differences or the different child-rearing practices across sub-cultures.
e.g. some USA samples were of middle-class pairings, while other USA samples used pairings from poorer socio-economic backgrounds.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Cultural Variations in Attachment: Further Research

A

Grossmann and Grossmann (1991) studied German infants and found that these infants were more likely to be classified as insecurely attached, specifically insecure-avoidant.

17
Q

how could Grossmann and Grossmann (1991) study be explained

A

This may be due to different child-rearing practices, as German culture involves keeping some interpersonal distance between parents and children.
Therefore, German infants do not engage in proximity-seeking behaviours in the Strange Situation in the same way that American infants were found to in Ainsworth’s original study and appear to be insecurely attached, despite perhaps being securely attached according to German standards.

18
Q

:( Nation rather than culture

A

The meta analysis drew conclusions about cultural differences yet they were not comparing cultures but countries.
Within each country, there are many different subcultures, each of which may have different child-rearing practices, which was not acknowledged by van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg. This means that comparisons between countries may have little meaning.

19
Q

:(Sample bias -> lowers the population validity of the findings

A

van Ijzendoorn’s research can be criticised with regard to the size of samples drawn from each of the 8 countries, as some countries were poorly represented within their meta-analysis. For example, there was only one study reviewed from China, with a small sample of 36 infants.
Also, many cultures were not represented at all in their meta-analysis – no samples were drawn from any African or South American countries.

20
Q

:( Culture bias in attachment theory

A

Rothbaum et al. (2000) argued that attachment theory is not relevant to other cultures because it is so rooted in American and British culture.
For example, the continuity hypothesis does not have the same meaning in America and Japan:
Bowlby (British) and Ainsworth (American) proposed that securely attached infants develop into more socially and emotionally competent children and adults.
However, social and emotional competence is defined in terms of individuation i.e. being able to explore, being independent and able to regulate one’s own emotions.
On the other hand, in Japan, competence is represented by the inhibition of emotional expression (not showing feelings) and being group-oriented rather than self-oriented.
Therefore, Bowlby’s theory fails to account for different cultural norms. Arguably, Anglo-American theories and assessments should not be applied to other cultures.

21
Q

Cross-cultural studies can suffer from an imposed etic

Imposed etic means that researchers use techniques e.g. the Strange Situation, designed in one culture but imposed on another, to study and/or draw conclusions about human behaviour.

A

In the Strange Situation (designed by an American and based on a British theory) it is assumed that willingness to explore is a sign of secure attachment. However, in traditional Japanese culture dependence is the sign of secure attachment.

The result of using an imposed etic to measure attachment is that Japanese children may appear to be insecurely attached according to Western criteria, whereas they are securely attached by Japanese standards.

This means that research using the Strange Situation may lack validity when assessing attachment types cross-culturally.

22
Q

Aim of Takahashi (1990) research

A

To assess whether the Strange Situation is a valid procedure when assessing the attachment types of children from other cultures, specifically Japanese children.

23
Q

Procedure of Takahashi (1990) research

A

60 middle-class, Japanese infants, aged 1 year, both boys and girls, and their mothers were assessed using the Strange Situation.

24
Q

Findings of Takahashi (1990) research

A

Securely Attached - 68%
Insecure-resistant - 32%
Insecure-avoidant - 0%
Separation anxiety: The Japanese infants were particularly distressed on being left alone; their response was so extreme that for 90% of the infants the study was stopped at this point.

25
Q

Conclusions: Takahashi (1990)
What could explain the high levels of separation anxiety?

A

This may be because Japanese infants rarely experience separation from their mothers i.e. they generally sleep with their parents until 2+ years of age, are carried around on their mothers’ backs and bathe with parents.
This would explain why they were more distressed in the Strange Situation than their American counterparts, as it would have been more than mildly stressful for them. Therefore, the behaviours observed were reactions to extreme stress.

26
Q

Conclusions: Takahashi (1990)
What does this suggest about the validity of the Strange Situation as a measure for assessing the attachment types of children in cultures outside of America?

A

This suggests that the Strange Situation does not have the same meaning for the Japanese as it does for American participants. Therefore, it is arguably not a valid measure for assessing the attachment types of infants in that culture.
Child-rearing practices need to be closely examined first in order to interpret findings based on the Strange Situation, as it is evident from Takahashi’s research that children may be incorrectly classified if not

27
Q

:( Evaluating Takahashi’s research
Ethical Issues

A

Research with children, especially infants, needs to be careful in terms of potential psychological harm to participants.
The Strange Situation was more than mildly stressful for Japanese infants.
Takahashi showed sensitivity by stopping the observations when infants became too distressed. However, the study itself was not stopped, even though it became obvious that extreme distress was likely.

28
Q

:( Evaluating Takahashi’s research
Sample bias

A

Takahashi used a limited sample of only middle-class, home-reared infants. It may, therefore, not be appropriate to generalise these findings to all Japanese infants.