Criminal Flashcards
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - aims
To investigate whether memory could be affected by post-event information, specifically leading questions
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - first experiment - procedure
Opportunity sample of 45 students were shown traffic accident footage (from safety films for drivers) that ranged from 5-30 seconds long. Participants were divided into five groups and asked the critical question ‘about how fast were the cars going when they - into each other’ with the gap being filled by one of five verbs (smashed, collided, bumped, hit and contacted). Mean speed estimates were then measured
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - first experiment - findings
Smashed had the highest mean speed at 40.5 mph, 9.7mph than contacted which had the lowest
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - first experiment - conclusion
That post-event information can influence the recall of that event, however a question remained over whether it actually changed the memory or just biased the response. This led to the second experiment
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - second experiment - procedure
150 participants were shown a one minute film, which included a short scene of a car accident. Three groups, one asked how fast when the cars ‘smashed’, another ‘hit’ and the other was not asked to estimate speed. A week later the participants were asked back and given questions about their recall of the clip, including ‘did you see any broken glass?’
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - second experiment - findings
The participants in the ‘smashed’ condition gave the highest estimates of speed and were most likely to report broken glass (there was none). 16 reported it in the ‘smashed’, 7 in the ‘hit’ and 6 in the control
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - second experiment - conclusion
The findings suggested that post-event information did not simply elicit a response bias but actually changed the memory of the event.
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - strengths
Well controlled procedure - lab study, only thing changed was critical question, same questionnaire and video
Application to changes in the legal system - the Devlin report was introduced to reccommend that EWT was not solely used to convict
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - strengths
Well controlled procedure - only change the participants experienced was the wording of he critical question. High internal validity as extraneous variables are reduced.
Application to changes in the legal system - Devlin report was published in 1976, recommending that juries should not convict solely from an eye witness testimony
Loftus and Palmer (1974) - weaknesses
Low ecological validity - watching a video of a car crash is very different from actually seeing one (paying full attention, less emotional response)
Sampling issues - all were college students, level of education may affect results, also all young so may not have experience driving.
Contradictory evidence - Yuille and Cutshall (1986)
Yuille and Cutshall (1986)
Interviewed witnesses to a real life shooting in Canada, thirteen witnesses were reinterviewed by the researchers four to five months later. They gave accurate accounts of the event even though the researchers deliberately included two leading questions
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour
Labelling
Self-fulfilling prophecy
Social learning
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - social learning
Criminal behaviour is learnt through the observation and imitation of others. To learn the behaviour it must be attended to, recalled and the would be offender must have the skill to perform it successfully. They must also be motivated to reproduce the behaviour, this could be due to vicarious reinforcement
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - social learning - strengths
Anderson and Gill (2000)
Harris and Klebold (1999)
Anderson and Gill (2000)
Participants who played violent video games were more likely to administer a loud and prolonged blast of noise to an opponent than those that played non-violent video games
Harris and Klebold (1999)
There is evidence to suggest that the people responsible for the Columbine high school massacre were influenced by violent films and video games (natural born killers)
Social explanations for crime and ant-social behaviour - weaknesses
Hard to test as it is unethical to make people commit crimes, therefore most research is due to case studies or generalised.
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - self-fulfilling prophecy
The idea that labels influence future behaviour. Labels would change the way that others treat an individual, leading to that individual internalising the label, eventually becoming ‘real’ as it influences the way that they act. This can be applied to the criminal label ‘deviant’, as they are treated differently and isolated from society, they would socialise more with deviant groups that feel the same way, confirming their label.
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - self-fulfilling prophecy - strengths
Jahoda (1954)
Rosenthal and Jacobsen (1968)
Jahoda (1954)
Studied the Ashanti of Ghana where they boys are named after the day that they are born. Monday boys (Kwadwo) were thought to be more even tempered than the Wednesday boys (Kwadku). Jahoda observed that the Kwadku were over three times more likely to be involved in violent crime than the Kwadwo over a five year period
Rosenthal and Jacobsen (1968)
Teachers at a primary school were told that a group of children were ‘spurters’ and were about to ‘bloom’ (they were chosen at random). At the end of the school year those children showed significant improvements in IQ than the children labelled as ‘standard’
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - self-fulfilling prophecy - weaknesses
Zebrowitz et al (1998)
There is not much evidence applying directly to crime, most of it is generalised. It is only assumed that it will work
Zebrowitz et al (1998)
Boys with a ‘baby face’ were more likely to be involved in crime, suggesting that they were overcompensating for appearing ‘weak’ or ‘childlike’ rather than living up to that label
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - labelling
A theory of how we classify ourselves and others using labels, which then define that person. For example, if a person is labelled deviant, it would come to define them affecting how people behave towards them.
Becker (1963)
Becker (1963)
Interested in how and why some actions become labelled as criminal. He believes that powerful groups in society create deviance by making up rules and applying them to people they see as outsiders. Crime is a social construct (no behaviour is inherently criminal)
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - labelling - strengths
Application to reintegrative shaming - Braithwaite (1989)
Rosenthal and Jacobsen (1968)
Social explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - weaknesses
Partial explanation of offending - the theory suggests that without labelling crime would not exist, and people that have not been labelled are not criminals. Most people would argue that murderers are criminals whether labelled or not
Evidence is generalised - it is not known whether labelling works directly with crime as most studies are undertaken in educational settings and generalised to crime
Braithwaite (1989)
Societies have lower crime and reoffending rates if they communicate shame about crime effectively. If the offender receives support and is helped back into society with the guidance of family and friends, they are less likely to reoffend
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour
Brain injury
Amygdala
XYY syndrome
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - brain injury
Williams et al (2010)
There are two types of brain injury acquired brain injuries (ABIs that are damage to a developing brain causing it to misfire) and traumatic brain injuries (TBIs which directly cause brain damage). The pre-frontal cortex does not fully develop until 25 and is an area that could easily be damaged when falling. Therefore they may not be able to move beyond the reckless and risk taking behaviour that is associated with childhood, making offending more likely.
Williams et al (2010)
Analysed data from 196 inmates from a single UK prison. 60% recalled a history of childhood head injuries, this group tended to be younger at the time of their first offence and recorded higher rates of reoffending
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - brain injuries - strengths
Raine et al (1997)
Williams et al (2010)
Fazel et al (2011)
Fazel et al (2011)
Analysed data from the Swedish population register from 1979 to 2009. They examined he association between traumatic brain injury and convictions for violent crime. Of those with an ABI 8.8% had committed a violent crime compared to 3% in a matched control group of similar size
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - brain injury - weaknesses
Low generalisability of Williams et al (2010) - all males, only one prison
Fazel et al (2011) - correlational study, people with brain injuries may be predisposed to other factors that lead to crime
Raine - less activity does not necessarily mean that that area is damaged
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - amygdala
The amygdala controls our emotional response to a stimulus (right excitatory, left inhibitory)
Part of the limbic system, it is key in fear conditioning and learning and normal function involves a balanced emotional response
Increased activity in the right amygdala will lead to increased impulsive violent behaviour
Damage or poor development can lead to issues with fear conditioning, children may fail to learn negative consequences of criminal behaviour
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - amygdala - strengths
Charles Whitman
Raine et al (1997)
Pardini et al (2014)
Charles Whitman
Had a tumour on his amygdala, leading to a noticeable change in behaviour with him becoming more aggressive and shooting people
Pardini et al (2014)
Conducted neuro imagery scanning on a group of 26 year old men. The men were divided based on the size of their amygdala (normal or reduced). When the researchers returned to them three years later the reduced group were three times more likely to be aggressive, violent or show psychopathic traits.
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - amygdala - weaknesses
Complex relationship between the amygdala and crime - other brain areas, such as the orbito frontal cortex, involved. It is thought to regulate self-control, reducing aggression
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - XYY
Around one in a thousand males are born with an extra Y chromosome, leading to them generally being taller, having lower intelligence and being more impulsive. Some studies show a link between XYY and crime.
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - XYY - strengths
Jacobs et al (1965)
Jacobs et al (1965)
Conducted a chromosome survey of males at a state hospital in Lanarkshire. Finding that men with XYY were overrepresented in the prison with 9/315.
Biological explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - XYY - weaknesses
Re and Birkhoff (2015)
Re and Birkhoff (2015)
A meta-analysis looking at 50 years of evidence and concluding that there is no link between XYY and crime. Larger numbers of XYY in prisons may be better explained through social factors
Personality explantions for crime and anti-social behaviour
Eysenck’s theory of criminal personality
Eysenck’s theory of criminal personality
Eysenck’s theory was that it is possible to measure personality along two dimensions
Extraversion - Introversion - extraverts crave excitement and stimulation, therefore they are more likely to engage in risk taking and dangerous behaviour, as well as tending not to condition easily
Neuroticism - Stability - neurotic individuals are nervy and anxious, their general instability makes them difficult to predict
Eysenck thinks that the criminal personality it Extrovert, Neurotic
Eysenck’s theory of criminal personality - biological basis
Personality traits that we develop are explained by the type of nervous system that we inherit. The constant need for excitement can be attributed to an underactive nervous system that requires high levels of arousal.
Eysenck’s theory of criminal personality - the third dimension
In later writings Eysenck added psychoticism as a new dimension, being seen in individuals that are self-centred, cold and lack empathy for others. A criminal personality would have a high psychoticism score
Eysenck’s theory of criminal personality - the socialisation process
Despite being biological in nature, in most people socialisation as a child will determine whether they become law-abiding or not. However, the fact that extraverts are less receptive to operant conditioning and therefore less affected by punishment can have an affect.
Personality explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - strengths
Boduszek et al (2013)
Raine et al (1990)
Application to early crime prevention - if criminal tendencies can be identified early in childhood, intervention can be used to modify the socialisation experiences of high risk offenders.
Personality explanations for crime and anti-social behaviour - weaknesses
Farrington et al (1982)
Not a single personality type that is criminal - Digman (1990)
Boduszek et al (2013)
Investigated the prevalence of Eysenck’s personality traits in repeat offenders (133 violent, 179 non-violent male prisoners). Suggested that the criminal personality had high levels of psychoticism, extraversion and neuroticism.
Raine et al (1990)
Took psychological measures from participants aged 15 years old and related these to later criminal status. Those with a criminal record 24 years later had recorded more signs of under-arousal in the nervous system than non-criminals. This suggests a link between biological factors and offending
Farrington et al (1982)
Found very little evidence that Eysenck’s questionnaire was an adequate measure for predicting offending in juveniles or adults. Suggests that Eysenck’s theory may lack validity