Chapter 5 Flashcards

1
Q

What are two other names for respondent conditioning?

A

In this chapter, we describe basic principles and procedures of respondent conditioning, also called PAVLOVIAN conditioning (after Ivan Pavlov, the Russian physiologist who studied it), or CLASSICAL conditioning (because it was the first type of conditioning to be identified). We use these three terms interchangeably.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

What are three characteristics of respondent behavior?

A

Some of our behaviors, like Susan’s feelings of fear while falling on her double-axel jump, seem to be reflexive and are called respondent behaviors, which are behaviors that are

(a) elicited by prior stimuli and are not affected by their consequences;
(b) referred to as involuntary; and
(c) usually involve smooth muscles and glands.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Define unconditioned reflex. Give three examples.

A

Examples include salivating when food is in your mouth, sweating when exposed to high temperatures, and experiencing a pounding heart when falling. Respondent behaviors are influenced by respondent conditioning, defined later, which is based on unconditioned reflexes. An unconditioned reflex is a stimulus–response relationship in which a stimulus automatically elicits a response apart from any prior learning. In other words, unconditioned reflexes are “hard wired” or inborn.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

State the principle of respondent conditioning. Clearly diagram an example of respondent conditioning that is not diagramed in the text and that uses one of the unconditioned reflexes in Table 5.1.

A

The principle of respondent conditioning states that if an NS (the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony) is followed closely in time by a US (food in the mouth) that elicits a UR (salivation), then the previous NS (the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony) will also tend to elicit the response of salivation in the future. Of course, it may take more than just one pairing of the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony with food before it would elicit any noticeable amount of salivation. Figure 5.1 illustrates respondent conditioning.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Define conditioned reflex and describe an example.

A

A conditioned reflex is a stimulus–response relationship in which a stimulus elicits a response because of prior respondent conditioning. If a salivation response were in fact conditioned to the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony, that stimulus–response relationship would be referred to as a conditioned reflex.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Define and give an example of the following: unconditioned stimulus, unconditioned response, conditioned stimulus, and conditioned response.

A

A stimulus that elicits a response without prior learning or conditioning is called an unconditioned stimulus (US). A response elicited by an unconditioned stimulus is called an unconditioned response (UR). In other words, an unconditioned reflex consists of a US and a UR. In the case of Susan, falling was a US, and Susan’s anxious feeling was a UR. Examples of unconditioned reflexes are listed in Table 5.1. The stimulus in a conditioned reflex is called a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony), defined as a stimulus that elicits a response because that stimulus has been paired with another stimulus that elicits that response. The response in a conditioned reflex is referred to as a conditioned response (CR; e.g., salivation to the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fi h Symphony), defined as a response elicited by a conditioned stimulus. In the case of Susan, approaching the take-off position for the double axel became a CS eliciting an anxious feeling as a CR. You can see how respondent conditioning might explain your reactions to certain words (such as the name of a loved one) or even to a single letter (such as “F” on an exam). Other examples include blushing when told that your fly or blouse is undone or becoming sexually aroused when watching X-rated movies.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Briefly, in a sentence each, describe five variables that influence the development of a conditioned reflex.

A
  1. First, the greater the number of pairings of a CS with a US, the greater is the ability of the CS to elicit the CR, until a maximum strength of the conditioned reflex has been reached.
  2. Second, stronger conditioning occurs if the CS precedes the US by about half a second, rather than by a longer time or rather than following the US.
  3. Third, a CS acquires greater ability to elicit a CR if the CS is always paired with the US than if it is only occasionally paired with the US.
  4. Fourth, when several neutral stimuli precede a US, the stimulus that is most consistently associated with the US is the one most likely to become a strong CS.
  5. Fifth, respondent conditioning will develop more quickly and strongly when the CS or US or both are intense rather than weak.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Diagram an example of higher-order conditioning.

A

Suppose that someone is conditioned to salivate to the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony by pairing it with food many times. The opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony will thus have become a CS for salivation. Now let’s suppose that, over several trials, just before presenting the opening motif of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony by itself (i.e., without following it with food), we turn on a yellow light. e light is a neutral stimulus for salivation and is never paired with food. However, a er a number of pairings of the light with the music (an established CS for the response of salivation), the light itself will come to elicit salivation. The procedure in which a neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus by being paired with another conditioned stimulus, instead of with an unconditioned stimulus, is known as higher-order conditioning. The pairing of the music with the food is referred to as conditioning of the first order. Pairing the light with the music is referred to as conditioning of the second order. Although third-order conditioning has been reported (Pavlov, 1927), higher-order conditioning beyond the second order appears to be difficult. The model for higher-order conditioning is presented in Figure 5.2.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Describe three examples of conditioned reflexes, one for each of these categories: digestion, circulation, and respiration.

A

In addition to salivation, a stomach reaction, feelings of nausea, and defecation are digestive responses that are susceptible to Pavlovian conditioning. A student experiences butterflies in the stomach (a CR) before giving a talk in class (a CS). A er experiencing chemotherapy, some patients feel nauseous (a CR) while waiting in the treatment room for therapy to begin (a CS). An extremely frightening experience, such as being threatened by a knife-wielding intruder (a CS), can cause anxiety defecation (a CR). Increased heart rate and blood flow are involved in many conditioned reflexes. Feeling embarrassed in a social setting, overhearing risqué topics discussed, having socially inappropriate thoughts—are all CSs for blushing in many individuals as blood flows to the outer layers of the skin. e circulatory system is involved when a scary scene in a movie or novel causes your heart to pound and when nude photographs elicit increased heart rate and increased blood flow to the genitals. Pavlovian conditioning has also been implicated in influencing coughing, sneezing, and asthma attacks—reflexes of the respiratory system. Suppose a person’s aunt, who visits rarely, happens to visit when that person suffers an asthma attack. e aunt might become a CS for the coughing and wheezing characteristic of an asthmatic reaction. Dekker and Groen (1956) reported that asthmatic responses have been elicited by such CSs as the sight of horses, caged birds, goldfish, and police vans.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

*Describe an example of a conditioned taste aversion.

A

A teenager drank several ounces of lemon gin in a short period of time. Approximately an hour later, he became violently ill. Since that time, the smell or taste of lemon gin causes instant nausea, and influences the teenager to avoid lemon gin.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

*Why do you suppose that we have evolved so that we are susceptible to conditioned taste aversion?

A

Having evolved so that we are susceptible to conditioned taste aversion likely had survival value for our ancestors. If, early in evolution, some humans evolved such that conditioned taste aversion occurred, those individuals were more likely to subsequently avoid eating substances that might kill them. If they had to learn to taste dangerous substances several times before learning to avoid them, repeated consumption of such substances might have been enough to cause death.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

What is biological preparedness? Give an example.

A

Seligman (1971) coined the term biological preparedness to refer to the predisposition of members of a species to be more readily conditioned to some neutral stimuli as CSs than to others. As an example of biological preparedness, humans will more quickly learn fears to stimuli, such as snakes and insects, that posed a threat to our survival than to stimuli, such as pictures of flowers, that were nonthreatening in our distant ancestors’ history. Conditioned taste aversion is another example of biological preparedness. Having evolved the strong tendency for taste to be conditioned to nausea greatly decreases the chances that one will repeatedly consume food that causes illness and perhaps death.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Discuss, with examples, whether all stimuli are equally capable of becoming CSs.

A

Now suppose that the parent takes the child to a dog show. Although there are lots of large dogs around, they have been trained to walk and sit quietly while on display. Repeated contact with these dogs (without them being paired with barking) will help the child overcome the fear of seeing dogs. at is, the sight of dogs loses its capability of functioning as a CS to elicit the fear reaction as a CR. Many of the fears that we acquire during childhood—fears of needles, the dark, lightning, and so on—undergo respondent extinction as we grow older, due to repeated exposure to these things in the absence of dire consequences. Figure 5.3 illustrates respondent extinction.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

State the procedure and result of the principle of respondent extinction. Describe an example that is not in this chapter.

A

The principle of respondent extinction involves the procedure of presenting a CS while withholding the US, with the result that the CS gradually loses its capability of eliciting the CR. Suppose that a child reaches out to touch a large dog just as the dog barks very loudly, scaring the child. As a function of the pairing of the loud bark with the sight of the big dog, the sight of the dog alone now elicits crying and trembling. This is a Pavlovian conditioned response that we label fear.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

*Describe the process of counterconditioning. Describe or diagram an example of counterconditioning that is not in this chapter.

A

A CR may be eliminated more effectively if a new response is conditioned to the CS at the same time that the former CR is being extinguished. For example, suppose that a young gymnast has learned to fear being on the balance beam because of a number of bad falls. Counterconditioning might involve having the young gymnast lick an ice cream cone (that would elicit responses to compete with fear) while standing on the beam.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Define respondent stimulus generalization. Give an example.

A

Respondent stimulus generalization occurs when an organism has been conditioned so that a particular CS elicits a CR, and then a similar stimulus also elicits that CR. Suppose, for example, while the dentist is drilling a cavity you have, you experience considerable pain while sitting in the dentist’s chair. e sound of the dentist’s drill is likely to become a CS causing you to cringe. Later, while visiting the meat market and hearing the sound of the butcher’s cutting machine, you feel yourself cringing. You are illustrating an example of respondent stimulus generalization.

17
Q

Define respondent stimulus discrimination. Give an example.

A

However, if you were to experience several repeated episodes of the dentist’s drill paired with some pain, and several repeated episodes of the butcher’s cutting tool never being paired with pain, then you would eventually show respondent stimulus discrimination—a stimulus functions as a CS to elicit a CR because that stimulus has been paired with a US that elicits that CR, but a similar stimulus does not function as a CS for that CR because the second stimulus has been paired with extinction trials.

18
Q

In the example of Susan learning to fear performing the double-axel jump, what was the US, UR, CS, and CR?

A

The psychologist taught Susan a relaxation technique referred to as deep-center breathing, which involves breathing low down in the stomach rather than high up in the chest. Deep-center breathing is a US that produces feelings of relaxation as a UR. Also, each time that Susan practiced deep-center breathing and while she was slowly exhaling on each breath, she was encouraged to say “reeeee-laaaaax” slowly to herself. “Reeeee-laaaaax” there- fore became a CS for feeling relaxed as a CR.

19
Q

*Describe (or diagram) how counterconditioning was involved to help Susan overcome her fear of attempting a double-axel jump.

A

Susan first established the word “relax” as a CS for feeling relaxed (as a CR) by saying “relax” slowly to herself during the exhaling part of deep center breathing. Then, while approaching the take-off position for her double axel (which had become a CS for fear as a CR) Susan told herself to “relax”. This helped her to relax while approaching the take-off position, which counter-conditioned the feeling of fear to that stimulus.

20
Q

Describe a respondent conditioning procedure for treating constipation. Identify the US, UR, CS, and CR.

A

An example of respondent conditioning of a desirable response is the treatment for chronic constipation developed by Quarti and Renaud (1964). Defecation, the desired response in cases of constipation, can be elicited by administering a laxative. However, reliance on such drugs to achieve regularity is not the healthiest solution because of undesirable side effects. Quarti and Renaud had their clients administer a distinctive electrical stimulus—a mild, nonpainful electric current—to themselves immediately prior to defecating. Defecation (UR) was initially elicited by a laxative (US), and then the amount of the drug was gradually decreased until defecation (CR) was elicited by the electrical stimulus (CS) alone. en, by applying the electrical stimulus at the same time each day, several of the clients were eventually able to get rid of the electrical stimulus, because the natural environment stimuli characteristically present at that time each day acquired control over the behavior of defecating. us, these clients achieved regularity without the continued use of a laxative.

21
Q

Describe a respondent conditioning component of a procedure for treating nocturnal enuresis. Identify the US, UR, CS, and CR.

A

Another example of respondent conditioning of a desirable response is the bell-pad treatment for nocturnal enuresis. One possible explanation for bed wetting, a problem that is rather common in young children, is that pressure on the child’s bladder while asleep and needing to urinate does not provide sufficient stimulation to awaken the child. A device that seems to be effective for many enuretic children consists of a bell connected to a special pad under the bottom sheet on the child’s bed. e apparatus is wired so that the bell sounds (US) and awakens (UR) the child as soon as the first drop of urine makes contact with the pad. Eventually, in many cases, the child will awaken before urinating because the response of waking up (now a CR) has been conditioned to the stimulus of pressure on the bladder (a CS). When that happens, the behavioral sequence of getting up, walking to the bathroom, and urinating in the toilet should be encouraged. But this latter sequence involves a type of learning called operant conditioning, rather than respondent conditioning.

22
Q

What are three characteristics of operant behavior? Give an example.

A

But much of our behavior is referred to as operant behavior—behavior that

(a) affects or “operates on” the environment to produce consequences, and which is, in turn, influenced by those consequences;
(b) is referred to as voluntary; and
(c) usually involves skeletal muscles. Examples include putting gas in your car, asking for directions, writing an exam, turning on a computer, and making breakfast.

23
Q

What is operant conditioning? Give an example.

A

Operant conditioning is a type of learning in which behavior is modified by its consequences. For example, through operant conditioning, we have learned to turn on a water tap to produce water and not to touch a hot stove because of the pain following past instances of doing so.