Chapter 4 - Behaviour and Attitudes Flashcards

1
Q

attitude

A

A favourable or unfavourable
evaluative reaction toward something
or someone, exhibited in one’s beliefs, feelings, or intended behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
1
Q

Implicit Association Test (IAT)

A

A computer-driven assessment of
implicit attitudes that uses reaction
times to measure people’s automatic
associations between attitude objects
and evaluative words, where easier
pairings (and faster responses)
are taken to indicate stronger
unconscious associations.

  • Implicit biases are pervasive. For example, 80 percent of people show
    more implicit negativity toward the elderly compared with the young.
  • People differ in implicit bias. Depending on their group memberships, their conscious attitudes, and the bias in their immediate environment, some people exhibit more implicit bias than others.
  • People are often unaware of their implicit biases. Despite thinking themselves unprejudiced, even the researchers exhibit some implicit biases (negative associa-
    tions with various social groups).
  • Implicit biases can harm. Implicit biases toward Indigenous people in Canada can lead to their not receiving necessary life-saving health care (Wylie & McConkey, 2019).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

When attitudes specific to behaviour are examined

A

reported Fishbein and Ajzen, in 26 out of 27 such research studies, attitudes did not predict behaviour. But attitudes did predict behaviour
in all 26 studies they could find in which the measured attitude was directly pertinent to the situation. Thus, attitudes toward the general concept of “health fitness” poorly predict specific exercise and dietary practices, but an individual’s attitudes about the costs and benefits of jogging are a fairly strong predictor of whether that person jogs regularly.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Theory of Reasoned Reaction

A

knowing people’s intended behaviours and subjective norms (in other words, what we think other people think about our behaviour). Later Ajzen added the concept of
perceived self-efficacy and control (Figure 4–1) which further validated the theory (and changed the name to the Theory of Planned Behaviour). Moreover, four dozen experimental
tests confirm that inducing new intentions induces new behaviour (Webb & Sheeran, 2006). Even simply asking people about their intentions to engage in a behaviour increases its likelihood (Levav & Fitzsimons, 2006). Ask people if they intend to floss their teeth in the next two weeks or to vote in an upcoming election, and they will become more likely to do so.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Forging strong attitudes through experience

A

When attitudes are forged by experience, not just by hearsay, they are more accessible, more enduring, and more likely to guide actions (Fazio & Zanna, 1981; Glasman & Albarracin, 2006). In one study, university students all expressed negative attitudes about their school’s response to a housing shortage. But, given opportunities to act (to sign a petition, solicit signatures, join a committee, or write a letter), only those whose attitudes
grew from direct experience acted (Regan & Fazio, 1977).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

role

A

A set of norms that define how
people in a given social position
ought to behave.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

norms

A

Rules for accepted and expected behaviour that prescribe
“proper” behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Prison experiment

A

Recreated prison, some people were prisoners and some were guards by chance. The guards began to disparage the prisoners, and some devised cruel and degrading routines. The prisoners broke down, rebelled, or became apathetic. There developed, reported Zimbardo (1972), a “growing confusion between reality and illusion, between
role-playing and self-identity . . . This prison which we had created . . . was absorbing us as creatures of its own reality.”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

gender roles

A

Behaviour expectations
(norms) for males and females.

In an experiment with undergraduate women, Mark Zanna and Susan
Pack (1975) showed the impact of gender role expectations. The women
answered a questionnaire on which they described themselves to a man
they expected to meet—a man they were told was tall, unattached, and
a fourth-year student. Those led to believe that the man’s ideal woman
was home-oriented and deferential to her husband presented themselves as more traditionally feminine than did women expecting to meet a man who liked strong, ambitious women. Moreover, given a problem-solving test, those expecting to meet the nonsexist man behaved more intelligently: They solved
18 percent more problems than those expecting to meet the man with the traditional views. This adapting of themselves to fit the man’s image was much less pronounced if the man was less desirable—a short, already attached first-year student. In a companion
experiment by Dean Morier and Cara Seroy (1994), men similarly adapted their self-presentations to meet desirable women’s gender role expectations. Clearly, our gender roles can shape our actions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

When saying becomes believing

A

People often adapt what they say to please their listeners. They are quicker to tell people good news than bad, and they adjust their message toward the listener’s position

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

foot-in-the-door phenomenon

A

The tendency for people who have
first agreed to a small request to
comply later with a larger request.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

low-ball technique

A

A tactic for getting people to agree to something. People who agree to an initial request will often still comply when the requester ups the ante. People who receive only the costly request are less likely to comply with it.

Cialdini and his collaborators found that this technique indeed works.
When they invited introductory psychology students to participate in an experiment at 7:00 a.m., only 24 percent showed up. But if the students first agreed to participate without knowing the time and only then were asked to participate at 7:00 a.m., 53 percent came.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

door-in-the-face technique

A

A strategy for gaining a concession.
After someone first turns down a
large request (the door in the face),
the same requester counteroffers with a more reasonable request.

The door-in-the-face technique works through

the principle of reciprocity. The basic idea is that an initial large request is presented—one that is so large that people will almost all say no (e.g., “Can you donate $100 for cancer research?”). The requester acquiesces and then makes a smaller request (“Well, if you can’t donate $100, how about $10?”). We feel bad about saying no at first so we say yes to the second request to “be nice.” Cialdini and his colleagues (1975) have shown that this “request then moderation” procedure is very effective at gaining compliance.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

immoral and moral acts

A

The attitudes-follow-behaviour principle works with more immoral acts as well. Such acts sometimes result from gradually escalating commitments. An early (seemingly innocuous) negative behaviour can make it easier for us to perform a worse act later. But these acts
gnaw at the actor’s moral sensitivity.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

toy robot experiment - moral actions

A

Researchers have tested character by
giving children temptations when it seems no one is watching. Consider what happens when children resist the temptation. They internalize the conscientious act if the deter-
rent is strong enough to elicit the desired behaviour yet mild enough to leave them with a sense of choice. In a dramatic experiment, Jonathan Freedman (1965) introduced elementary-school children to an enticing battery-controlled robot, instructing them not to play with it while he was out of the room. Freedman used a severe threat
with half the children and a mild threat with the others. Both were sufficient to deter the children.
Several weeks later, a different researcher, with no apparent relation to the earlier events, left each child to play in the same room with the same toys. Of the 18 children who had been given the severe threat, 14 now freely played with the robot, but two-thirds of those who had been given the mild deterrent still resisted playing with it. Having earlier made a conscious choice not to play with the toy, the mildly deterred children
apparently had internalized their decision. This new attitude controlled their subsequent action. Thus, moral action, especially when chosen rather than coerced, affects moral thinking.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

cognitive dissonance theory

A

Tension that arises when we are simultaneously aware of two inconsistent cognitions. For example, dissonance may occur when we realize that we have, with little justification, acted contrary to our
attitudes or made a decision favouring one alternative despite reasons favouring another.

14
Q

Why action effects attitude

A

(1) Self-presentation theory assumes that, for strategic reasons, we express attitudes that make us appear consistent;

(2) cognitive dissonance theory assumes that to reduce discomfort, we justify our actions to ourselves

(3) self-perception theory assumes that our actions are self-revealing
(when uncertain about our feelings or beliefs, we look to our behaviour, much as anyone else would). Let’s examine each explanation.

14
Q

insufficient justification

A

Reduction of dissonance by internally justifying one’s behaviour when external justification is “insufficient.”

they would experience more discomfort (dissonance) and thus be more motivated to believe in what they had done.

Festinger and his student, J. Merrill Carlsmith (1959).

experiment where you turn doorknobs again and again for a while
people are asked then to tell the person waiting outside how exciting and interesting the experiment was
they were either didn’t need to lie, were given a dollar, or 20 dollars

then they had to personally rate the experiment

  • people who didn’t have to lie personally rated the experiment low
  • people who were given 20 dollars rated the experiment medium - they said the dull experiment was interesting, but they had sufficient reasoning for doing so
  • people who were given one dollar rated the experiment the highest - they said the dull experiment was interesting, they did not have too good of reasoning for it, maybe it was sort of interesting
15
Q

Why do actions effect attitudes - self perception theory (self-observation)

A

The theory that, when unsure of our attitudes, we infer them much as would someone observing us—by looking at our behaviour and the circumstances under which it occurs.

16
Q

Why do actions effect attitudes - self justification (cognitive dissonance theory)

A

Tension that arises when we are simultaneously aware of two inconsistent cognitions. For example, dissonance may occur when we realize that we have, with little justification, acted contrary to our
attitudes or made a decision favouring one alternative despite reasons favouring another.

17
Q

Why do actions effect attitudes - self presentation theory (impression management)

A

We present ourselves more intelligently when speaking to authority figures
Our wanting to present a desired image both to an external audience and to an internal audience
We are reinforcing how we see ourselves

18
Q

overjustification effect

A

The result of bribing people to do what they already like doing; they may then see their action as externally controlled rather than intrinsically appealing.

19
Q

Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

A

When people do something they enjoy, without reward or coercion, they attribute their behaviour to their love of the activity. External rewards undermine intrinsic motivation
by leading people to attribute their behaviour to the incentive.

20
Q

self-affirmation theory

A

A theory that people often experience self-image threat after engaging in an undesirable behaviour, and they compensate for this threat by affirming another aspect of the self. Threaten people’s self-concept in one domain, and they will compensate either by refocusing or by doing good deeds in some other domain.

study where one group of students were forced to right an essay and another group of students had the illusion that they had the choice to write the essay
- then they all had to take a pill (it actually was just powdered milk)
- One-third of the students were told that the pill would make them feel aroused, one-third were told that it would make them feel relaxed, and one-third were given no information
about the effects of the pill.

  • the people who were told that that they would feel aroused by the pill did not change their attitude about writing the essay
  • the people who were told it had no effect experienced regular dissonance
  • the people who were told the pill would make them feel relaxed experienced the most attitude change

Self-affirming also seems to help people let down their guard and pay attention to health messages—researchers have found that providing opportunities to self-affirm allows people to be more receptive to messages about quitting smoking