Chapter 12 Flashcards
Spoken word recognition
Phonological lexicon: a store of the abstract speech sounds that make up known words.
Lexical access: the process of matching a perceptual description of a word on to a stored memory description of that word.
Recognizing spoken words: The Cohort Model
- All candidates considered in parallel.
- Candidates eliminated as more evidence becomes available in the speech input.
- Uniqueness point occurs when only one candidate remains.
- Uncommon words activated less (speed > species).
- However, semantic context (e.g. reference to Darwin) does not alter the pattern.
- Suggests semantics occurs late (i.e. after spoken word recognition).
Evidence for a late influence of semantics comes from N400 ERPs.
Semantic memory
Semantic memory represents our conceptual knowledge of the world – the meaning of words and objects, factual knowledge (e.g. Paris is the capital of France). Has a central role in human cognition in that it lies at the interface of language, memory and perception.
Models of semantic memory
All models propose that concepts are comprised of a constellation of constituent features, models of semantic memory differ in terms of:
- What format do the features take: e.g. amodal versus grounded?
- How are the features organised: hierarchical versus non-hierarchical?
- Is category information (e.g. “is an animal”) represented in addition to feature-level information (e.g. “has eyes”), or are categories purely emergent properties of features?
Grounded/embodied concepts
E.g. Barsalou (2008)
- Amodal representations (= independent of input/output modality): Symbol grounding problem.
- Concepts not defined in terms of each other, but in terms of our experiences and interactions with the world
E.g. concepts of “green” and “kick” are linked to sensory and motor experiences rather than abstract/amodal representations.
- Not necessarily linked to innate knowledge, but rather to shared experience.
- What about abstract concepts? A problem for fully-grounded models, but some abstract concepts can be explained: (1) claim that number and time concepts are processed spatially, (2) emotions may be embodied feeling states linked to certain contexts/stimuli.
Example of fully-grounded semantics
Allport (1985): Concepts widely distributed in the brain with different concepts drawing on different features.
Functional imaging:
Regions involved in perception also role in naming/memory.
The hub-and-spoke model
Patterson et al. (2007)
- Amodal semantic hub (anterior temporal lobes).
- Grounded/embodied semantics (rest of brain).
Semantic dementia: damage to hub
Spokes sustain typical feature probabilities, but not knowledge of exceptions. Struggle with atypical category members.
Models of semantic memory
All models propose that concepts are comprised of a constellation of constituent features, models of semantic memory differ in terms of:
- What format do the features take: e.g. amodal versus grounded?
- How are the features organised: hierarchical versus non-hierarchical?
- Is category information (e.g. “is an animal”) represented in addition to feature-level information (e.g. “has eyes”), or are categories purely emergent properties of features?
Collins and Quinlan’s model - Amodal and hierarchical
Distance effects (e.g. faster in classifying robin as bird than as animal BUT
- Distance effects could depend on frequency of co-occurrence rather than hierarchy (e.g. robin; bird).
- Not all concepts have a hierarchical relationship (e.g. truth, justice, law).
Sub-ordinate and super-ordinate information in the brain (different substrates)
Rogers, et al. (2006)
fMRI study of naming and categorisation; processing at specific level activates anterior temporal pole (same region as in semantic dementia) but other levels activate posterior temporal lobes. Patients with ATL damage (struggle with atypical category members) retain ability in superordinate classification, but struggle more with item and subordinate-level classifications.
Evidence for animate-inanimate category specificity
- Warrington and McCarthy (1983) – patient with good knowledge of animals, foods, flowers, relative to inanimate objects.
- Warrington and Shallice (1984) – four patients with the opposite profile.
- Impairments found at comprehending pictures and words, naming pictures, and matching pictures with words (i.e. an amodal deficit).
- Explained using the sensory–functional distinction.
- Animals defined by sensory properties, whereas inanimate objects, in particular tools, defined by function.
Evidence against sensory-functional distinction
- Some patients with category-specific impairments don’t show a difference between knowledge of sensory vs. functional facts.
- Some patients have selective difficulties in comprehending sensory properties but don’t have category-specific impairments.
- Some patients with particularly selective deficits for one category (e.g., food).
Suggests categories and features are represented separately (and can be damaged separately) rather than one being based upon the other.
Putting words into sentences - role of syntax and semantics
Parsing = putting words into sentences.
A = The boy hit the girl.
B = The girl hit the boy.
C = The girl was hit by the boy.
A & B have different meaning but same syntax.
A & C have same meaning but different syntax.
Broca’s area and syntax
Previously believed that Broca’s aphasia (and Broca’s area) was related to speech production and Wernicke’s area (and Wernicke’s aphasia) related to speech comprehension.
From 19th-century to 1970s:
- Broca’s aphasia linked to symptom of agrammatism rather than failure of speech production more generally.
- Agrammatism = halting speech production that is devoid of function words, bound morphemes and often verbs.
A changing view in 1970s: Broca’s area = syntactic processor involved in both sentence production and comprehension. Broca’s aphasia associated with problems with comprehending sentences in a picture-sentence matching task. Reversible & semantically improbable: subject and object of the verb are determined from syntax and not from semantics.
Broca’s area & syntax: modern perspectives:
- Early evidence depended on diagnosing Broca’s aphasia by symptom rather than anatomy.
- Recent studies which accurately map damage show Broca’s region important for syntax (and healthy fMRI).
- Also for non-language syntax.
- BUT it is not the only region that is: temporal lobes involved as implicated by fMRI and lesion studies.
- Processing of sentences, comprising real words, shows increasing activity in a region of broca’s area according to the degree of syntactic complexity.
Broca’s area: beyond syntax:
- Fractionation into posterior and anterior regions.
- Posterior regions: complex syntax.
- Anterior regions: working memory & complex semantics.