CH. 2. The Neural Basis For Cognition Flashcards
Capgras Syndrome
CAPGRAS SYNDROME – Someone with this syndrome is fully able to recognize the people in her world — her husband, her parents, her friends — but is utterly convinced that these people are not who they appear to be.
- Often, a person with Capgras syndrome insists that there are slight differences between the “impostor” and the person he (or she) has supposedly replaced — subtle changes in personality or appearance.
-
What is going on here? The answer lies in the fact that facial recognition involves two separate systems in the brain.
- One system leads to a cognitive appraisal (“I know what my father looks like, and I can perceive that you closely resemble him”), and the other…
- a more global, emotional appraisal (“You look familiar to me and also trigger a warm response in me”).
- When these two appraisals agree, the result is a confident recognition (“You obviously are my father”).
-
In Capgras syndrome, though, the emotional processing is disrupted, leading to an intellectual identification without a familiarity response
- Someone with this syndrome is able to recognize a loved one’s face, but with no feeling of familiarity.
- Brain scans suggest a link between Capgras syndrome and abnormalities in several brain areas, indicating that our account of the syndrome will need to consider several elements.
- One site of damage in Capgras patients is in the temporal lobe, particularly on the right side of the head. This damage probably disrupts circuits involving the AMYGDALA – an almond-shaped structure that — in the intact brain — seems to serve as an “emotional evaluator,” helping an organism detect stimuli associated with threat or danger.
Amygdala and the Two-System Hypothesis
AMYGDALA – an almond-shaped structure that — in the intact brain — seems to serve as an “emotional evaluator,” helping an organism detect stimuli associated with threat or danger.
- The amygdala is also important for detecting positive stimuli indicators of safety or of available rewards.
- With damaged amygdalae, therefore, people with Capgras syndrome won’t experience the warm sense of feeling good (and safe and secure) when looking at a loved one’s familiar face.
- This lack of an emotional response is probably why these faces don’t feel familiar to them, and is fully in line with the TWO-SYSTEM HYPOTHESIS – which requires 1) a cognitive response AND 2) an emotional response in order to recognize and accept the identity of a loved one in Capgras syndrome.
- With damaged amygdalae, therefore, people with Capgras syndrome won’t experience the warm sense of feeling good (and safe and secure) when looking at a loved one’s familiar face.
Prefrontal Cortex
PREFRONTAL CORTEX – is an area in the Frontal Lobe especially active when a person is doing tasks that require PLANNING or careful ANALYSIS.
- Conversely, this area is less active when someone is dreaming – reflecting the absence of careful analysis of the dream material, which helps explain why dreams are often illogical or bizarre.
Note on Capgras Syndrome:
- Patients with Capgras syndrome also have brain abnormalities in the frontal lobe, specifically in the right prefrontal cortex.
- With fMRI scans of patients suffering from schizophrenia – neuroimaging reveals diminished activity in the frontal lobes whenever these patients are experiencing hallucinations (same effect as with dreams).
- One interpretation is that the diminished activity reflects a decreased ability to distinguish internal events (thoughts) from external ones (voices) or to distinguish imagined events from real ones.
- With damage to the frontal lobe, Capgras patients may be less able to keep track of what is real and what is not, what is sensible and what is not. As a result, weird beliefs can emerge unchecked, including delusions (about robots and the like) that you or I would find totally bizarre.
Recognition and the Two-System Hypothesis
TWO-SYSTEM HYPOTHESIS – RECOGNITION of ALL STIMULI (not just faces) involves TWO SEPARATE MECHANISMS:
- one that hinges on factual knowledge
- one that’s more emotional
- Capgras syndrome teaches us that many different parts of the brain are needed for even the simplest achievement.
- In order to recognize your father, for example,
- Facts – one part of your brain needs to store the factual memory of what he looks like. Another part of the brain is responsible for analyzing the visual input you receive when looking at a face. Yet another brain area has the job of comparing this now-analyzed input to the factual information provided from memory, to determine whether there’s a match.
- Emotions – Another site provides the emotional evaluation of the input.
- Combination – A different site presumably assembles the data from all these other sites — and registers the fact that the face being inspected does match the factual recollection of your father’s face, and also produces a warm sense of familiarity.
- If coordination among these areas is disrupted – yet another area works to make sure you offer reasonable hypotheses about this disconnect and not zany ones. (In other words, if your father looks less familiar to you on some occasion, you’re likely to explain this by saying, “I guess he must have gotten new glasses” rather than “I bet he’s been replaced by a robot.”)
- Unmistakably, this apparently easy task — seeing your father and recognizing who he is — requires multiple brain areas. The same is true of most tasks, and in this way, Capgras syndrome illustrates this crucial aspect of brain function.
- NOTE: That our understanding of Capgras syndrome depends on a combination of evidence drawn from cognitive psychology and from cognitive neuroscience.
- Just as both perspectives can illuminate Capgras Syndrome, both can be illuminated by the syndrome.
- That is, we can use Capgras syndrome (and other biological evidence) to illuminate broader issues about the nature of the brain and of the mind.
- Ex: Capgras syndrome suggests that the AMYGDALA (the “Emotional Evaluator”) plays a crucial role (the emotional side) in supporting the feeling of familiarity.
- That is, we can use Capgras syndrome (and other biological evidence) to illuminate broader issues about the nature of the brain and of the mind.
- This observation gives us a way to think about occasions in which your evaluation of the facts points toward one conclusion, while an emotional evaluation points toward a different conclusion.
The Brain
THE BRAIN:
- The human brain weighs (on average) a bit more than 3 pounds (1.4 kg)
- Male brains weighing about 10% more than female brains
- Estimated to contain 86 billion nerve cells
- 860 trillion connections
- Different parts of the brain perform different jobs.
- Symptoms produced by brain damage depend heavily on the location of the damage.
- Therefore, we need to understand brain functioning with reference to brain anatomy
- The human brain is divided into three main structures: the hindbrain, the midbrain, and the forebrain.
Hindbrain, Midbrain, Forebrain
The human brain is divided into three main structures: the hindbrain, the midbrain, and the forebrain.
HINDBRAIN – is located at the back of the brain at the very top of the spinal cord and includes structures crucial for controlling key life functions.
- Helps to regulate autonomic functions (Breathing, Heart rate)
- Relay sensory information from the body to the brain.
- Coordinate movement
- Maintain balance and Equilibrium
MIDBRAIN in the middle part of the brain and wrapped completely by the Forebrain:
- Helps to coordinate movements
- Processes auditory information
- Processes visual information
- Regulate the experience of pain
FOREBRAIN – This structure surrounds (and so hides from view) the entire midbrain and most of the hindbrain.
- Processes sensory information
- Helps with reasoning and problem-solving
- Regulates autonomic functions (involuntary actions like heart rate and breathing)
- Regulates endocrine functions (Hormones and Biological Processes)
- Regulates motor functions (Glands, reflexes, muscle contractions)
Hindbrain
HINDBRAIN – is located at the back of the brain at the very top of the spinal cord and includes structures crucial for controlling key life functions.
- Regulates the rhythm of heartbeats and the rhythm of breathing.
- Maintains the body’s overall tone.
- Helps maintain the body’s posture and balance.
- Helps control the brain’s level of alertness.
Other parts of the HINDBRAIN include:
-
CEREBELLUM – the largest area of the hindbrain. Its role is to:
- coordination of bodily movements and balance.
- spatial reasoning
- discriminating sounds
- integrating the input received from various sensory systems
- PONS – (Latin for “bridge”) is the main connection between the cerebellum and the rest of the brain.
- MEDULLA – controls vital functions such as breathing and heart rate.
Midbrain
MIDBRAIN – Has several functions:
- Coordinates Movements including the precise movements of the eyes
- Relays auditory information from the ears to the forebrain for processing and interpretation.
- Regulates the experience of pain
Forebrain
FOREBRAIN – the most interesting brain region (and the largest in humans – 80% of the brain by volume.) The forebrain surrounds (and so hides from view) the entire midbrain and most of the hindbrain. It is comprised of:
-
CEREBRAL CORTEX – the outer surface of the forebrain. A thin covering on the outer surface of the forebrain; on average, it’s a mere 3 mm thick.
- Cortex refers to an organ’s outer surface, and many organs each have their own cortex. what’s visible in the drawing, then, is the cerebral cortex.
- As thin as it is, it consists of a large sheet of tissue crumpled up and jammed into the limited space inside the skull.
- It’s this crumpling that produces the brain’s most obvious visual feature — the wrinkles, or CONVOLUTIONS.
- Some of the “valleys” between the wrinkles are actually deep grooves (called FISSURES) that divide the brain into different sections, called LOBES.
- The deepest groove is the LONGITUDINAL FISSURE, running from the front of the brain to the back, which separates the left cerebral hemisphere from the right.
- Frontal Lobes – form the front of the brain, right behind the forehead.
- Parietal Lobes – the brain’s topmost part.
- Temporal Lobes – are below the lateral fissure
- Occipital Lobes – are found at the very back of the brain, connected to the parietal and temporal lobes.
- Central Fissure – divides the frontal lobes on each side of the brain
- Lateral Fissure – bottom edge of the frontal lobes
Subcortical Structures
SUBCORTICAL STRUCTURES – (meaning BELOW the CEREBRAL CORTEX) – there are several subcortical structures that are hidden from view underneath the cortex. They are:
- THALAMUS – acts as a relay station for nearly all the sensory information going to the cortex.
- HYPOTHAlAMUS – a structure that plays a crucial role in controlling behaviors that serve specific biological needs — behaviors that include eating, drinking, and sexual activity.
-
LIMBIC SYSTEM –This set of structures surrounds the thalamus and hypothalamus and includes a number of subcortical structures that play a crucial role in learning and memory and in emotional processing.
- Included in the LIMBIC SYSTEM are the Amygdala & Hippocampus. Both the amygdala & Hippocampus are located under the Cortex.
- These two structures are essential for learning and memory:
-
AMYGDALA – plays a key role in emotional processing.
-
Ex: The presentation of frightful faces causes high levels of activity in the amygdala.
- Likewise, people ordinarily show more complete, longer-lasting memories for emotional events, compared to similar but emotionally flat events.
- This memory advantage for emotional events is especially pronounced in people who showed greater activation in the amygdala while they were witnessing the event in the first place.
- Conversely, the memory advantage for emotional events is diminished (and may not be observed at all) in people who (through sickness or injury) have suffered damage to the amygdalae.
-
Ex: The presentation of frightful faces causes high levels of activity in the amygdala.
- HIPPOCAMPUS – a brain structure embedded deep into the temporal lobe with a major role in learning and memory.
-
AMYGDALA – plays a key role in emotional processing.
Lateralization & Contralateral Connections
LATERALIZATION – Virtually all parts of the brain come in pairs, left and right, including structures (like the hippocampus and the amygdala) and lobes
- In all cases, the left and right structures in each pair have roughly the same shape and the same pattern of connections to other brain areas.
- However, there are differences in function between the left-side and right-side structures.
- That said, the two halves of the brain work together – the functioning of one side is closely integrated with that of the other side.
-
COMMISSURES – Connect the two hemispheres. These are what make integration possible. They are thick bundles of fibers that carry information back and forth between the two hemispheres.
-
CORPUS CALLOSUM – is the largest commissure.
- In certain cases, though, there are medical reasons to Sever the Corpus Callosum and some of the other commissures. Someone who had this procedure done is said to be a “SPLIT_BRAIN PATIENT”
-
CORPUS CALLOSUM – is the largest commissure.
-
“SPLIT_BRAIN PATIENT“ – still having both brain halves, but with communication between the halves severely limited.
- Research with these patients has taught us a great deal about the specialized function of the brain’s two hemispheres.
- It is misleading to claim that we need to silence our “left-brain thinking” in order to be more creative – some elements of creativity depend on specialized processing in the right hemisphere
- Even so, whether we’re examining creativity or any other capacity, the two halves of the brain have to work together, with each hemisphere making its own contribution to the overall performance.
- Therefore, “silencing” one hemisphere wouldn’t allow you new achievements because the skills we display depend on the whole brain.
- Hemispheres are not cerebral competitors, they are an integrated team.
-
COMMISSURES – Connect the two hemispheres. These are what make integration possible. They are thick bundles of fibers that carry information back and forth between the two hemispheres.
CONTRALATERAL CONNECTIONS – This refers to the fact that the corresponding area in the right hemisphere receives its input from the left side of the body and the left hemisphere from the right side of the body.
-
VISUAL – Likewise for the visual projection areas, although here the projection is NOT contralateral with regard to body parts. Instead, it’s contralateral with regard to physical space.
- Specifically, the visual projection area in the right hemisphere receives information from both the left eye and the right, but the information it receives corresponds to the left half of the visual space.
- The reverse is true for the visual area in the left hemisphere. It receives information from both eyes, but from only the right half of visual space.
- AUDITORY – The pattern of contralateral organization is also evident — although not as clear-cut with auditory signals.
Neuropsychology
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY – the study of the brain’s structures and how they relate to brain function.
CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY – Looks at damaged brains to understand the functioning of undamaged brains.
Lesion
LESION – (a specific area of damage on the brain). Clinical Neuropsychologists look at lesions (damage) on specific parts of the brain to determine what that specific area is used for.
- The damage tends to inhibit or disable the function of that specific part of the brain revealing the primary function of that particular area.
- Ex: A lesion in the occipital cortex produces problems in vision but spares the other sensory modalities.
- The consequences of brain lesions depend on which hemisphere is damaged. These patterns provide a rich source of data that helps us develop and test hypotheses about those functions.
Neuroimaging Techniques
NEUROIMAGING TECHNIQUES – produce precise, three-dimensional pictures of a living brain.
-
MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING (MRI scan) – Shows Brain Structure – neuroimaging procedures that provide structural imaging, generating a detailed portrait of the shapes, sizes, and positions of the brain’s components.
- MRI scans provide structural images using magnetic properties of the atoms that make up brain tissue.
- CT SCAN – Computerized Axial Tomography. CT scans rely on X-rays to show the structure of the brain (as does an MRI)
-
FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING (fMRI) – Shows Brain Activity by location – tells us the specific areas of activity (as well as the levels of activity) in the brain in response to some stimuli or activity.
- fMRI scans offer an incredibly precise picture of the brain’s moment-by-moment activities.
- PET SCAN – positron emission tomography. PET scans use radiation to determine the activity levels in specific areas of the brain (as does an fMRI).
- Structural imaging (CT or MRI scans) are relatively stable, changing only if the person’s brain structure changes (From injury or disease).
-
Activity imaging (PET or fMRI scans), in contrast, are highly variable, because the results depend on what task the person is performing.
- Thus, PET and fMRI are used to explore brain function.
- Ex: to determine which brain sites are especially activated when someone is making a moral judgment or trying to solve a logic problem.
- Thus, PET and fMRI are used to explore brain function.
COMBINING TECHNIQUES – as with most things, combining different techniques allows us to get a fuller picture of the situation. This is because the strengths of one approach often fill in for the weaknesses of another approach.
- CT scan and MRI data tell us about the shape and size of brain structures, but they tell nothing about the activity levels within these structures.
- PET scans and fMRI studies do tell us about brain activity, and they can locate the activity rather precisely (within a millimeter or two). But these techniques are less precise about when the activity took place.
- fMRI data summarize the brain’s activity over a period of several seconds and cannot indicate when exactly, within this time window, the activity took place.
- EEG data give more precise information about timing but are much weaker in indicating where the activity took place.
- So COMBINING the:
- Structural information provided by the CT/MRI with the
- Activity information provided by the PET/fMRI and the
- Timing provided by the EEG…
- …will give us the most accurate picture of what’s going on.
Neuron & Neurotransmitter
NEURONS – are nerve cells. The brain contains billions of them.
- It is the neurons that do the brain’s main work.
- They communicate with one another via chemical signals called NEUROTRANSMITTER.
- Once a neuron is “activated,” it releases the transmitter, and this chemical can then activate (or, in some cases, de-activate) other, adjacent neurons.
- The adjacent neurons “receive” this chemical signal and, in turn, send their own signal onward to other neurons.
-
Neurons have an “input” end and an “output” end.
- The “input” end is the portion of the neuron that’s most sensitive to neurotransmitters.