C9 concepts Flashcards
Define concept
A general idea which groups similar things, events, people. Applies to every member of a category.
A category being what is thought about (the concept is the thinking)
Words are used to communicate thoughts/concepts. Some words can be ambiguous - multiple meanings (fair/fare)
Concepts are unambiguous - if it appears to relate to two different categories it’s actually two different concepts.
Everyday concepts can be taken for granted but can develop over a long period of time. Categorisation can be very important - difference in legal status between an adult and child. Clinical diagnosis is an example of categorisation.
Categorization
Bruner - the ability to treat individual things in terms of their group membership - implies similar behaviour towards different instances of the same category.
Techniques used to study categorisation - sorting tasks - some people class eggs as breakfast food, others as dairy produce. property listing tasks - write down properties of concept (eg dog) frequency indicates how central property is to the concept.
Concepts and cognition
Make it easier to remember information
Enable semantic classification - similar to perceptual classification used in recognition
May be units of semantic memory that store facts (compared to episodes/experiences stored in episodic memory)
We store relationships between concepts (cats are animals) and lexical concepts in the mental lexicon may be used to link words with their meanings.
Concepts enable inferences/assumptions to be made
Explaining categorisation - theories
Classical theory: things with the same properties belong to the same category;
Prototype theory: things that have “most” of the properties of a category belong to it;
‘Theory’ theory: instances of a category have the same explanations;
Psychological essentialism: members of categories share some essential properties (even if we can’t identify these).
Classical and prototype theory
Both classical and prototype theory consider that category membership of an item depends on how similar (to different degrees) it is to other members of the category, i.e. how “typical” the item is of that category;
This approach has been successful to some extent:
Hampton (1998) found that typicality explained most (46-96%) of the variance in how likely an item was to be judged as a member of a category in borderline cases;
However other predictors such as lack of familiarity of the participant with the item/category, as well as “technical membership’ (e.g. a dolphin is “technically” a mammal but judged to be superficially more like a fish) were almost as good predictors.
This suggests that deeper analysis is involved, not just comparison of external properties/features, leading to other explanations such as “theory” theory and psychological essentialism.
Classical theory claims
That items belong to the same category if they share certain properties, if they don’t have all the properties they are not members of that category (necessary condition)
If an item has all the properties it must be a member of that category (sufficient condition)
Classical theory evidence for
Empirical studies (Hull, 1920; Bruner et al., 1956) showed people do associate common properties with category members
Classical theory evidence against - Typicality
Typicality - eg a robin is a more typical bird than a penguin.
Rosch showed all or nothing doesn’t work - people think of some items as more typical than others - verified robin is a bird sentences faster than penguin is a bird
Suggest categories have rich internal structure which classical theory doesnt explain
Classical theory evidence against - Borderline cases
when does red become orange? - doesnt fit with all or none
McCloskey and Glucksberg found people rated items (e.g. bookends as furniture or not) inconsistently both as individuals at different times, and between individuals. This might have been due to lack of knowledge, but probably not. However the classical theory would claim that items are category members or not.
Classical theory evidence against - Intransitivity
Classical theory claims category membership is transitive, e.g. if As are members of category B and members of category B are also members of category C, then As should also be members of category C;
However empirical studies show that people don’t categorise according to this rule: e.g. Hampton “Chairs are furniture; car seats are chairs but car seats are not furniture” (could be criticised on the basis that by “chairs” people mean “some chairs”)
Classical theory evidence against - Lack of definitions
Putnam (1975) claimed that we can retain belief in a category even if its definition changes completely - e.g. we would still call them “cats” if they were proven to really be robots controlled from Mars rather than mammals;
Because the defining characteristics of a category can change as we discover new information, even though we continue to believe that it’s still a category, the classical theory that all members of natural categories can be defined by the common properties they share doesn’t make sense.
Prototype theory claims
Concepts relate to the most typical example - the one that best matches a set of properties - not all or nothing like classical.
There is no necessary condition - can have many values but with typicality ratings (red apple more likely that yellow)
Prototype theory evidence against - Typicality effects in definitional concepts
Armstrong et al. (1983) found robust typicality effects even for concepts that conform to a definition, not just opinion such as “even numbers” some participants rated 4 as a more typical even number than 34.
Implies categories have an internal structure. So suggest a dual-process model for categorisation…
- A concept core is used for judgement of category membership (classical theory);
- A set of identification procedures are used to match possible instances to a category (prototype theory).
Prototype theory evidence against - Context sensitivity
Context affects how “typical” an instance is seen to be of a category
Medin and Shoben demonstrated that the influence of “large” and “small” on typicality was affected by the context of “wooden” or “metal” spoons
Prototype theory does not explain this instability effect - enough similar properties should be matched in each case for typicality to be equal, but this suggests that weightings are changing
Prototype theory evidence against - Complex concepts
Combinations of concepts (“red car”) are hard to explain as it’s difficult to theorise how prototypes could be combined