Aggression Flashcards

You may prefer our related Brainscape-certified flashcards:
1
Q

Neural mechanisms in aggression - the amygdala

A
  • our limbic system in our brain consists of a network of structures including the hypothalamus and amygdala.
  • the amygdala is the most important structure. It plays a key role in humans and animals and how they respond to their environment.

-Gospic et al. (201 1) used a lab method of assessing aggressive behaviour called the Ultimatum Game. This features two players. The Proposer offers to split money in a certain way with the Responder. If the Responder accepts, the money is split as proposed. But if the Responder rejects the offer, both receive nothing. Participants in this study played as Responders while having their brains scanned by fMRI, which highlights activity in different areas of the brain. The researchers found that when Responders rejected unfair offers (an aggressive reaction to a social provocation), scans revealed a fast and heightened response by the amygdala. They also found that a benzodiazepine drug (which reduces arousal of the autonomic nervous system) taken before the game had two effects on responses to unfair offers. It halved the number of rejections (i.e. reduced aggression) and decreased the activity of the amygdala. This is strong evidence of an association between reactive aggression and amygdala activity.

-serotonin is an inhibitory neurotransmitter it reduces neural activity leading to a reduction in self-control. this increases impulsive behaviours such as aggression. Virkkunen et al compared levels of serontonin breakdown product in the cerebrospinal fluid of violent impulsive and violent non-impulsive offenders. the levels were significantly lower in impusive offenders impiling their is some distruption of serontonin functioning imn agreession

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Hormonal mechanisms in aggression

A
  • the fact that men are more aggressive than women is linked to testosterone
  • it also has a role In regulating social behaviour
  • Dolan et al (2011) found a positive correlation between testosterone and aggression in 60 male offenders in uk maximum security hospitals
  • cortisol mediates other hormones such as testosterone
    -high levels of cortisol inhibit testosterone so inhibit aggression
  • low levels of cortisol= to high aggression in which a psychologist found low levels of cortisol in violent offenders
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Research support of neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression

A

Berman et al. (2009) gave their participants either a placebo or a dose of paroxetine, a drug which enhances serotonin activity. Participants then took part in a laboratory-based game in which electric shocks of varying intensity were given and received in response to provocation. The paroxetine participants consistently gave fewer and less intense shocks than those in the placebo group. However, this was only true of the participants who had a prior history of aggressive behaviour. Nevertheless this study is useful evidence of a link between serotonin function and aggression that goes far beyond the usual correlational findings.

However, Most research into both neural and hormonal influences on aggression is correlational. There are good ethical reasons for this, because opportunities to experimentally manipulate brain structures and hormones are very limited. But when two variables are correlated, it is impossible to establish which one is the cause of the other, or if a third variable is involved.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Genetic factors in aggression- A01 twin studiees and adoption studies

A

.
- Caccaro used adult twin pairs and found that 50% concordance rtae in MZ twins and 19% in DZ and for verbal aggression 28% MZ and 7% DZ
- Adoption studies= . If a positive correlation is found between aggressive behaviour in adopted children and their biological parents, a genetic effect is implied. If a positive correlation is found between the adoptee’s aggressive behaviour and the rearing family, then an environmental effect is implied.
- - Hutchings and Mednick conducted a study of over 14,000 adoptions in Denmark and found that a significant number of adopted boys with criminal convictions had biological parents with convictions providing evidence for genetic effects. This suggests that genes can have an impact on individuals

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Evaluation of genetic factors in aggression - Evidence of the MOAO gene

A

-McDermott et al. (2009) showed that participants with the low-activity MAOA gene behaved aggressively in a laboratory-based money-allocation game, but only when they were provoked. Otherwise, they were no more or less aggressive than other participants.

-Brunner et al. (1993) studied 28 male members of a large Dutch family who were repeatedly involved in impulsively aggressive violent criminal behaviours such as rape, attempted murder and physical assault. The researchers found that these men had abnormally low levels of MAOA in their brains and the low-activity version of the MAOA gene.
-Gregory Stuart et al. (2014) studied 97 men who, because they had been involved in inflicting intimate partner violence (IV), were part of a batterer treatment programme. Men with the low-activity MAOA gene were found to be the most violent perpetrators of IPV. They engaged in the highest levels of physical and psychological aggression and inflicted the worst injuries on their partners.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

The ethological explanation of aggression

A
  • looks at two functions of aggression:
  • adaptive= because the defeated animal is rarely killed but rather forced to establish territory elsewhere
  • establishment of dominance hierarchies= male chimpanzees use aggression to climb their social hierarchy and give them special status. Also seen in humans is group of young child playing. Aggressive behaviours = more dominance over other children
  • suggests aggression is innate and cannot be learnt
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Ritualistic aggression and IRM and FAPS

A
  • Lorenz pointed out that intra-species aggressive confrontations end with ritual appeasement displays (backing down). These indicate acceptance of defeat and inhibit (lower) aggressive behaviour in the victor, preventing any damage to the loser. For instance, at the end of an aggressive confrontation a wolf will expose its neck to the victor, deliberately making itself vulnerable to a single bite to its jugular vein. This is adaptive because if every aggressive encounter ended with the death of one of the combatants, that could threaten the existence of the species.
  • a ritual= series of behaviour carried out in a set order

Innate releasing mechanisms
- An innate releasing mechanism (IRM) is a built-in physiological process or structure e.g. a network of neurons a circuit) in the brain. An environmental stimulus (such as a certain facial expression) triggers the IRM which then ‘releases’ a specific sequence of behaviours. called a fixed action pattern (FAP).
- According to Stephen Lea (1984), FAPS have six main features. They are:
- stereotyped= unchanging
-universal
-unaffected by learning
-ballistic= once triggered cannot be stopped until completion
-single- purpose= behaviour only occurs in specific situations
-A response to an identifiable specific sign stimulus

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Research into IRM’s And FAPS

A
  • in male sicklebacks during mating season the presence of a red spot (developed during the mating season) triggers FAP.
  • Niko Tinbergen (1951) presenter sicklebacks with a series of wooden models of different shapes
    -finding: Regardless of shape, if the model had a red spot the stickleback would aggressively display and even attack it. But if there was no red spot, there was no aggression, even if the model looked realistically like a stickleback. Tinbergen also found that these aggressive FAPs were unchanging from one encounter to another. Once triggered, the FAP always ran its course to completion without any further stimulus.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Evaluation of the ethological explanation in aggression to do with evidence about other theories

A

-Brunner et al(1993) MAOA gene research. And research into activity in the limbic systems has been shown to trigger aggressive behaviour in humans and other animals. Increases the validity of the ethological explanation as it argues aggression is genetically determined and heritable

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

The evolutionary explanation of human aggression

A
  • evolutionary approaches explains aggression in terms of its ability to increase survival therefore would have been beneficial in early humans
  • male aggression can occur as a result of sexual jealousy.
    -Unlike women, men can never be entirely certain that they are the fathers of their children. As a result, men are at risk of cuckoldry, the reproductive cost that might be inflicted on a man as a result of their partners infidelity.
    • The consequence of cuckoldry is that the man might unwittingly invest his resources in offspring that are not his own. Therefore adapt aggressive behaviour to deter a mate from sexual infidelity.
  • Wilson and Daly (1996) identified mate retention strategies:
  • direct guarding= involves male vigilance over a partner’s behaviour, for example checking who they’ve been seeing, coming home early, keeping tabs on their whereabouts, installing tracking apps on their mobiles, etc.
    • Negative inducements, such as issuing threats of dire consequences for infidelity (“ I’ll kill myself if you leave me’).

women who reported mate retention strategies in their partners (they agreed with statements like ‘He insists on knowing who you are with and where you are at all times’), were twice as likely to have suffered physical violence at the hands of their partners. Of these women, 73% required medical attention and 53% said they feared for their lives.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Study for intimate partner violence

A

shackleford et al (2005) carried out this study.

Men and women in 107 married couples completed different questionnaires.
All of the participants had been married less than one year. The men completed the Mate Retention Inventory, which assessed mate retention behaviours in various categories (such as direct guarding). The women completed the Spouse Influence Report, which measured the extent of their partner’s violence in their relationship.

They found: There was a strong positive correlation between men’s reports of their mate retention behaviours and women’s reports of their partners’ physical violence. So men who used guarding (e.g. coming home early) or negative inducements (e.g. threats to kill) were more likely to use physical violence against their partners.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Evolutionary explanation of bullying

A
  • Volk et al. (2012) argue that the characteristics associated with bullying behaviour are attractive to the opposite sex. In males, it suggests dominance, acquisition of resources, and strength. It also has the benefit of warding off potential rivals. Bullying-associated characteristics therefore deliver the ideal combination of access to more females and minimal threat from competing males. Therefore such behaviour would be naturally selected because these males would have greater reproductive success.
    Female bullying more often takes place within a relationship and is a method of controlling a partner. Women use bullying behaviour to secure their partner’s fidelity. which means they continue to provide resources for future
    offspring. Again such behaviour would be naturally selected because of enhanced reproductive success.

Therefore bullying may have been used by ancestors as an adaptive strategy to increase chances of survival by creating opportunities for reproduction

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Evaluation of the evolutionary explanation of aggression to do with evidence and gender differences

A
  • shackleford et al study IPV study
    ALSO
    The evolutionary explanation explains gender differences in aggression such as why men are more physically aggressive than women .Campbell (1999) argues that a female with offspring is motivated to be less aggressive because such behaviour would put not only her own survival at risk but also that of her child. So a more adaptive strategy for females is to use verbal aggression as a means of retaining a partner who provides resources, and to avoid becoming involved in life-threatening situations involving physical aggression.therefore women are more likely to use non-aggressive methods of resolving disputes than men.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

The frustration aggression hypothesis

A

• Dollard et al (1939) developed the frustration-aggression hypothesis which argued that frustration always leads to aggression and aggression is always a result of frustration.
• Dollard explains that if the individual is prevented from achieving a goal by some external factor, then this will lead to frustration which will always lead to aggression.
• It is argued that the drive for aggression was innate, like the drive for food. He believed that the only way to reduce aggression is to engage in an activity which released it.
- the hypothesis recognises aggression is not always expressed directly against the source of frustration as the source of aggression may be abstract e.g. economic situation, the cause may be too powerful so we risk punishments e.g. teacher who gave you a low grade, the cause may be unavailable at the time. E.g. only realising teacher gave u a lower grade that you deserved once they left the room

  • talk about one research study
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Research into frustration aggression - jigsaw puzzle

A
  • Geen (1968) Male university students were given the task of completing a jigsaw puzzle. Their level of frustration was experimentally manipulated in one of three ways. For some participants, the puzzle was impossible to solve. For others, they ran out of time because another student in the room (a confederate of the researcher) kept interfering. For a third group, the confederate took to insulting the participant as they failed to solve the puzzle. The next part of the study involved the participant giving electric shocks to the confederate when they made a mistake on another task.

Findings :The insulted participants gave the strongest shocks on average, followed by the interfered group, then the impossible task participants. All three groups selected more intense shocks than a (non-frustrated) control group.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Evaluation of the frustration aggression model to do with research support

A

Marcus-Newhall et al. (2000) conducted a meta-analysis of 49 studies of displaced aggression, a key concept in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. These studies investigated situations in which aggressive behaviour had to be directed against a target other than the one that caused frustration.
The researchers concluded that displaced aggression is a reliable phenomenon. Participants who were provoked but unable to retaliate directly against the source of their frustration were significantly more likely to aggress against an innocent party than people who were not provoked. This is exactly the outcome predicted by the frustration-aggression hypothesis

HOWEVER
-Bushman (2002) found that participants who vented their anger by repeatedly hitting a punchbag actually became more angry and aggressive rather than less. In fact, doing nothing was more effective at reducing aggression than venting anger. Bushman argues that using venting to reduce anger is like using petrol to put out a fire. But this is exactly the advice many therapists and counsellors give to their clients.
The outcome of this study is very different from that predicted by the frustration-aggression hypothesis. This casts doubt on the validity of a central assumption of the hypothesis

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Social learning theory in aggression

A
  • suggests children acquire aggression via operant conditioning.if the aggressive act has a positive consequence it’s likely to be positively reinforced and repeated.
    Aggression can also be indirectly reinforced. (Vicarious reinforcement). They can learn this via aggressive models such as parents, peers, characters in media) if the models behaviour isn’t punished and rewarded= positive reinforcement.
  • cognitions also account for wether the behaviour is replicated. Mediational process: attention,retention, reproduction, motivation
  • self efficacy= individuals believe that their behaviour will attain a goal and must have confidence.this grows as a child learns that aggressive behaviour will bring a reward
  • explanation of Banduras bobo doll experiment…
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Evaluation of the social learning theory to with analysis of aggressive behaviour in young boys and cultural different

A

-François Poulin and Michel Boivin (2000) applied a social learning analysis to aggressive behaviour in boys aged between nine and twelve years. They found that most aggressive boys formed friendships with other aggressive boys. The researchers describe such cliques as ‘training grounds’ for antisocial behaviour. These friendships were lasting, stable, and mutually reinforcing of aggression. The boys used their alliances with each other to gain resources through aggressive behaviour, usually successfully. This means that they were exposed frequently to models of physical aggression (i.e. each other) and to the positive consequences of it, They also gained reinforcement from the rewarding approval of the rest of the ‘gang’. These are precisely the conditions under which social learning theory predicts that aggressive behaviour would occur.

HOWEVER a limitation
Different cultures have different norms about which behaviours should be reinforced. In some cultures, such as the Kung San of the Kalahari desert, direct reinforcement of children’s aggression is unlikely because social norms do not encourage it, and parents tend not to use it to discipline children. This also means that models of aggression are unavailable for children to observe, and certainly vicarious reinforcement is a rare experience.
Nevertheless they display aggressive behaviour.

-

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

De-individuatin

A

-A psychological state in which an individual loses their personal identity and takes on the identity of the social group. The result may be to free the individual from the constraints of social norms.
- talk about research
-Zimbardo (1969) distinguished between individuated and de-individuated behaviour. In an individuated state, our behaviour is generally rational and normative (i.e. it conforms to social norms). But de-individuated behaviours are emotional, impulsive and irrational
-We have less fear of retribution because we are a small and unidentifiable part of a faceless crowd; the bigger the crowd, the more anonymous we are.
- according to Dunn and rogers (1982) de-individuation creates a greater likelihood for aggression not because of anonymity but the consequences of anonymity such as
-Private self-awareness concerns how we pay attention to our own feelings and behaviour. This is reduced when we are part of a crowd. Our attention becomes focused outwardly to the events around us, so we pay less attention to our own beliefs and feelings. We are less self-critical, less thoughtful and less evaluative, all of which foster a de-individuated state.
-Public self-awareness refers to how much we care about what other people think of our behaviour, and this is also reduced in crowds. We realise that we are just one individual amongs many; we are anonymous and our behaviour is less likely to be judged by others. We no longer care how others see us, so we become less accountable for our aggressive and destructive actions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Research into de-individuation

A

Dodd (1985) was a psychology teacher who developed a classroom exercise to illustrate de-individuation

Procedure He asked 229 undergraduate psychology students in 13 classes this question: ‘If you could do anything humanly possible with complete assurance that you would not be detected or held responsible, what would you do?’ The students were aware that their responses were completely anonymous. Three independent raters who did not know the hypothesis decided which categories of antisocial behaviour the responses belonged to

Findings Dodd found that 36% of the responses involved some form of antisocial behaviour;
26% were actual criminal acts, the most common of which was ‘rob a bank’. A few students opted for murder, rape and assassination of a political figure. Only 9% of responses were prosocial behaviours (such as helping people). In terms of how people imagine they would behave, this study demonstrates a link between anonymity, de-individuation and aggressive behaviour.
Furthermore
-Spivey & Prentice-Dunn 1990 found Deindividuation can lead to either prosocial or antisocial behaviour depending on situational factors. Prosocial environmental cues such as a prosocial model encouraged deindividuated participants to perform altruistic acts and fewer antisocial acts.

This evidence decreases the value of the theory as de individuation may not always be a bad thing.

-Watson 1973 Anthropological evidence from 23 societies shows that those that change their appearance
more drastically before going to war killed, tortured or mutilated their victims more savagely.
Also
Douglas and McGarty(2001) found that more aggressive behaviour online was linked to anomanimity.
This has real life application as it can be used to stop cyber bullying by introducing features to allow more identification

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Evaluation of de-individuation to do with evidence

A

-Karen Douglas and Craig McGarty (2001) looked at aggressive online behaviour in chatrooms and uses of instant messaging. They found a strong correlation between anonymity and ‘flaming’, that is sending or posting threatening and/or hostile messages. The most aggressive messages were sent by those who chose to hide their real identities. This suggests the existence of a link between anonymity, de-individuation and aggressive behaviour in a context that has even greater relevance today. This is because of the explosion in social media use, the activities of online ‘trolls’, and the exit from Twitter of several high-profile media celebrities.
HOWEVER,
Some research studies show that de-individuation does not always lead to aggression. In their ‘deviance in the dark’ study, Gergen et al. (1973) selected groups of eight participants, who were all strangers to each other. They were placed in a completely darkened room for one hour, and told to do just whatever they wanted to, with no rules to stop them. It was impossible for the participants to identify one another, and they were given a guarantee that they would never encounter each other again. It did not take long for them to stop talking and start kissing and touching each other intimately. The study was repeated, but this time the participants were told they would come face-to-face with each other after the
“hour of darkness’. Unsurprisingly, the amount of touching and kissing declined dramatically. Of all the behaviours that de-individuation could have given rise to in this study, aggression was not one of them.

22
Q

The importation model ( dispositional explanation)

A

-the importation model claims that prison inmates bring their own traits and normative systems developed outside of the prison inside such as norms,beliefs, attitudes and personal attributes such as race,gender and class. This influences how they adapt to and behave in the prison environment; they are not “blank slates” when they enter. As Thomas and McManimon (2005) put ‘ people who prey on others in the street also prey on others in prision’. They do this to establish status and power in prison.

23
Q

The deprivation model ( situational)

A
  • the deprivation model claims Harsh prison conditions are stressful for inmates, who have to cope by resorting to aggressive and often violent behaviour. These conditions include being deprived of freedom, independence, goods and services, safety, and heterosexual intimacy. Deprivation of material goods is especially important because it increases competition amongst inmates to acquirenthem, and is accompanied by a corresponding increase in aggression.
    Aggression is also influenced by the nature of the prison regime. If it is unpredictable and regularly uses ‘lock ups’ to control behaviour, then this creates frustration, reduces stimulation by barring other more interesting activities, and reduces even further access to ‘goods’ (such as television). This is a recipe for violence, which becomes an adaptive solution to the problem of deprivation.
24
Q

Evaluation of the deprivation model- research support against + for importation model

A

-Scott Camp and Gerald Gaes (2005) studied 561 male inmates with similar criminal histories and predispositions to aggression. Half were placed in low-security Californian prisons and the other half in the second-highest category of prisons: 33% of prisoners in the low security prisons and 36% in the higher category prisons were involved in aggressive misconduct within two years, a difference that was not statistically significant. The researchers concluded that features of the prison environment are significantly less important predictors of aggressive behaviour than characteristics of inmates. This is strong evidence because the study is a field experiment with random allocation of inmates to prisons of different security levels, allowing more valid conclusions than correlational studies or natural experiments.

25
Q

The effects of computer games on aggression - experiential studies

A

-In a lab-based study by Bruce Bartholow and Craig Anderson (2002), students played either a violent computer game (Mortal Kombat) or a nonviolent game (PGA Tournament Golf for ten minutes. They all then carried out the Taylor Competitive Reaction Time Task (TCRTT), a standard laboratory measure of aggression in which the students delivered blasts of white noise at chosen volumes to punish a (non-existent) opponent.
Those who played the violent game selected significantly higher noise levels compared with the nonviolent players (means of 5.97 and 4.60 decibels respectively).

26
Q

Evaluation of the effects of computer game studies

A

-Laboratory experiments allow us to conclude that exposure to aggressive media (for example playing violent computer games)can cause aggression This is because exposure to aggressive media (the independent variable) is manipulated by the experimenter in conditions where other influences are controlled. However, Laboratory experiments may suggest a causal link between aggressive media and aggressive behaviour, but they tend to use measures of aggression that are very artificial and unrealistic. For example, choosing to subject another person to a loud blast of noise is not a form of aggression that is found in most real-life situations. Also unlike the real world, there is no fear of retaliation therefore in labs is unrealistic and lacks external validity.

-Many correlational studies also point towards a link between violent media and aggression. But these cannot establish the direction of causality because no IV is manipulated. Although it is possible that viewing violent media causes aggressive behaviour, it is more likely that people who are already aggressive choose to be exposed to violent media such as computer games. Or third factor may be the cause. Waffle….

A limitation of experimental and correlation they generally investigate the short-term effects of media aggression. Improving our understanding of these short-term effects is obviously desirable.
But it is also limitation because the effects of media on aggression are most often longer term. HOWEVR
Longitudinal studies take this in to account. They directly focus on the effects of media over a period of many years. This reflects the more realistic view of media influences as dynamic and changing, and of people as active users of media rather than passive recipients.so is more realistic
That being said, there is a chance of effects of confounding variables such as participants may drop out, or deaths….

  • scientific research tend to only publish findings which are statistically significant. Which is a problem for meta-Analyses, because they generally only include published studies.therefore perhaps not getting the full picture of results
27
Q

Desensitntion

A

-Normally when we witness violent actions we experience physiological arousal associated with the sympathetic nervous system, such as increased heart rate, higher blood pressure, greater sweat activity, and so on. But when children in particular repeatedly view aggression on TV or play violent computer games, they become habituated to its effects. We experience less physiological arousal and less anxiety making us more likely to be aggressive.
Similarly, constant exposure to aggression promotes the belief that violence is acceptable and we are less empathetic to victims and less likely to have negative views about aggression

Research:
-Monica Weis and Christopher Earls (1995). They showed their participants the feature film Straw Dogs, which contains a prolonged and graphic scene of rape. Participants then watched a re-enactment of a rape trial. Compared with those who watched a non-sexually violent film, male viewers of Straw Dogs showed greater acceptance of rape myths and sexual aggression.
They also expressed less sympathy towards the rape victim in the trial, and were less likely to find the defendant guilty. There was no such effect of film type on female participants.

28
Q

Disinhibition

A

-our normal restraints such as our negative view on aggression is loosened after exposure to media violence.
- aggressive behaviour becomes normalised and a aggressive response is acceptable. The social learning theory explains the indirect and direct learning of this behaviour. Media often rewards aggression such as the aggression person wins power,status and attention.Child see superhero’s as role models creating a social norm in children.
- Also seen in adult films such as James Bond which child may look up to and imitate.
The portrayal of aggression as justified can create norms that it is socially acceptable

29
Q

Cognitive priming

A

Repeated viewing of aggressive media, especially game playing, can provide us with a ‘script about how violent situations may ‘play out’. According to Rowell Huesmann (1998), this script is stored in memory, and so we become ‘ready’ or primed to be aggressive. The process is mostly automatic; it can direct our behaviour without us even being aware of it. The script is triggered when we encounter cues in a situation that we perceive as aggressive.
A study that illustrates the priming of aggressive scripts in memory was carried out by Peter Fischer and Tobias Greitemeyer (2006). They looked at the priming of aggressive scripts in memory by investigating a neglected form of media violence - song lyrics. Male participants listened to songs featuring aggressively derogatory lyrics about women. Compared with when they listened to neutral lyrics, participants subsequently recalled more negative qualities about women and behaved more aggressively towards a female confederate. This procedure was replicated with female participants, using ‘men-hating’ song lyrics, with similar results.

30
Q

Evaluation of desensitisation,disinhibition,cognitive priming

A

Krahd et al. (201 1) showed particinants violent (and nonviolent) film. clips while measuring physiological arousal using skin conductance (see page
264). Participants who were habitual viewers of violent media showed lower levels of arousal as they watched the violent rim clies. They also reported higher levels of pleasant arousal and lower levels of anxious arousal. Lower arousal was correlated with unprovoked (proactive) aggression in a ‘noise blast’ task.
These findings confirm hypotheses based on desensitisation. The lower arousal in violent media users reflects desensitisation to the effects of violence, and a greater willingness to be aggressive.

Research support for disinhibition

Leonard Berkowitz and Joseph Alioto (1973) found that participants who saw a film depicting aggression as vengeance gave more (fake) electric shocks of longer duration to a confederate. This suggests that media violence may disinhibit aggressive behaviour when it is presented as justified. This is because vengeance is a powerful justification for violence, and justified violence is more likely to be seen as socially acceptable.
This adds validity to the disinhibition concept because it demonstrates the link between removal of social constraints and subsequent aggressive behaviour, at least in the case of justified aggression.

  • disinhibition of aggression however is more general than specific.e.g. Children do not learn specific behaviours from cartoon models.( it is not possible to punch someone so their head spins around 360 degrees)

Practical application of cognitive priming

There are potentially life-saving benefits in understanding how cognitive priming influences aggression in real-life situations. Whether such situations break out into violence often depends on how the participants interpret environmental cues. This in turn depends on the cognitive scripts they have stored in memory.
According to Brad Bushman and Craig Anderson (2002), someone who habitually watches violent media accesses stored aggressive scripts more readily. This means they are more likely to interpret cues as aggressive, resort to a violent solution and fail to consider the alternatives.
This explanation provides a possible means by which violent media could trigger aggressive behaviour through the priming of cognitive scripts.
Effective interventions could potentially reduce aggressive behaviour by challenging hostile cognitive biases and encouraging habitual violent media users to consider alternatives to aggression, such as humour or negotiation.

31
Q

vResearch support for neural and hormonal mechanisms of aggression to Do with the role of other brain structures

A

Recent studies indicate that the amygdala does not operate in isolation in determining aggression. It appears to function with the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which is not part of the limbic system. The OFC is thought to be involved in self-control, impulse regulation and inhibition of aggressive behaviour Coccaro et al. (2007), in patients with psychiatric disorders that prominently feature aggression, activity in the OFC is reduced, disrupting its impulsive-control function and thus leading to increased aggression.
Combined with the findings of Gospic et al. (2011) this indicates that the regulation of aggression is highly complex and involves at least three neural structures in the brain: the amygdala, the OFC and the connection between the two.
- could be more than 3 more than three
Can a link really be established?

32
Q

Evaluation of neural and hormonal mechanism of aggression to do with explaining the role of testosterone

A

-Mazur (1985) created the biosocial model of status (BMoS) to explain the link between testosterone and aggression in humans. Testosterone levels change rapidly during the course of the day, e.g. in competition. Mehta and Josephs (2006) measured changes in their male participants* testosterone levels before and after a competitive game (which they all lost). Once the second sample was taken, the participants were given a choice.
They could either challenge their opponent who won to another competition (aggressive) or complete an unrelated task (non-aggressive). Of the losers whose testosterone levels rose after their loss, 73% rechallenged. Therefore showing only after an increase in testosterone levels = links to aggression. confirming Mazur’s BMoS, increasing its validity.

33
Q

Genetic factors in aggression- A01 the MOAO gene

A

MAOA gene is responsible for
MAOA ( monoamine oxidase) which is an enzyme which breaks down neurotransmitters especially serotonin. A dysfunction in the operation of this gene = abnormal MOAO levels which in turn affects levels of serotonin in the brain. Linking to aggression

34
Q

evaluation of genetic explanation aggression to do with issues of focusing on convicted criminals

A
  • problems with many studies focusing exclusively on individuals convicted of violent crimes. Convictions for violent crime are relatively few compared to the vast number of violent attacks by individuals that never result in a conviction. They therefore represent just a small minority of those regularly involved in aggressive behaviour. Arguably people named ‘violent’ in country are not necessarily the most serious offenders. This might explain why so many studies have found little or no evidence of heritability for violence. Therefore emailed not representative = issues with generalising
35
Q

evaluation of genetic explanations of aggression to do with metholodgy

A

-An issue with studies of aggression is that they have relied on either parental or self reports of aggressive behaviour, and others have used direct observations. In Rhee and Waldman meta-analysis of 51 adoption studies, genetic factors had a greater influence on aggression in studies using self-reports rather than parent or teacher
reports. If research findings vary depending upon how aggression is measured, then it becomes very difficult to draw valid conclusions about the role of genetic factors

36
Q

Evaluation of the ethological explanation of aggression to do with cultural difference

A
  • cultural difference in aggression: there is evidence that aggressive behaviour is more common in some human cultures than in others. For example, Richard Nisbett (1993) found there was a north-south divide in the United States for homicide rates. Killings are much more common amongst white males in the southern states than in the northern states. Because this was only true for reactive aggression triggered by arguments, Nisbett concluded that the difference in homicide rates was caused by a ‘culture of honour’, in other words the response to impuisive aggression was a learned social norm.

-This was supported in a lab study by Richard Nisbett and his colleagues (1996) who found that when white males from the south were insulted in a research situation, they were more likely than northern white males to become aggressive. It is difficult for ethological theory, with its view of aggression as instinctive, to explain how culture can override innate influences.

Only white and males used= issues in generalisability

37
Q

Evaluation of the ethological explanation of aggression - research support against

A

– Goodall (2010) of chimpanzees at the Gombe Stream National Park in Tanzania. During what she called the ‘four-year war, male chimps from one community set about systematically slaughtering all the members of another group. They did this in a co-ordinated and premeditated fashion.
On some occasions, a victim was held down by some rival chimpanzees while others hit and bit it in an attack lasting up to 2o minutes. The violence continued like this despite the fact that the victims were offering signals of appeasement and defencelessness. These signals did not inhibit the aggressive behaviour of the attacking chimps as predicted by the ethological explanation. ( research support against)

38
Q

Evolutionary explanation of aggression to do with application of bullying

A
  • Ken Rigby
    (2010) reviewed several established interventions based on the assumption that bullying is a maladaptive behaviour that can be eradicated by addressing the bully’s perceived deficiencies. Despite the availability of these strategies, bullying is still prevalent.
    The evolutionary approach acknowledges that bullies bully because they stand to gain advantages for themselves. So it would make no sense for them to voluntarily give up the power they have over others without some form of compensation. Volk et al. argue that anti-bullying interventions therefore need to increase the costs of bullying and the rewards of prosocial alternatives. One method could be to encourage bullies to compete aggressively but fairly in sporting activities. This would give them the opportunity to display prowess, strength and other attractive qualities, including some not available to them through bullying
39
Q

Research into t he frustration- aggression model- presence of aggressive cues

A

–Even if we become angry, we still might not behave aggressively. According to Leonard Berkowitz (1989), frustration merely creates a readiness for aggression.
But the presence of aggressive cues in the environment make acting upon this much more likely. Therefore cues are an additional element of the frustration-aggression hypothesis. Leonard Berkowitz and Anthony LePage (1967) demonstrated this in a famous study.
Procedure They arranged for student participants to be given electric shocks in a laboratory situation, creating anger and frustration. The individual who gave the shocks was a confederate of the researchers. The participants then had the opportunity to turn the tables and give electric shocks to the confederate.
Findings The number of shocks given depended on the presence or absence of weapons in the lab. In one condition, two guns were present on a table next to the shock machine. The average number of shocks given in this condition was 6.07. When no guns were present, the average number of shocks was significantly fewer, at 4.67.
This so-called weapons effect supports Berkowitz’s contention that the presence of aggressive environmental cues stimulates aggression. (The finger pulls the trigger, but the trigger may also be pulling the finger’, as he put it.)

40
Q

Evaluation of the frustration aggression model to do with the negative affect theory

A

It became clear from research that frustration does not always lead to aggression, and that aggression can occur without frustration. So the hypothesis was reformulated by Leonard Berkowitz (1989) who argued that frustration is just one of many aversive stimuli that create negative feelings; others include jealousy, pain, and loneliness. So aggressive behaviour is triggered by negative feelings generally (such as anger) rather than by frustration specifically. Furthermore, the outcome of frustration can be a range of responses, only one of which is aggression.
For example, the frustration someone experiences at getting a low grade for an essay might not necessarily lead to aggression but to despair, anxiety, helplessness or determination.
Berkowitz’s negative affect theory arose because the original frustration-aggression hypothesis was inadequate, as it could only explain how aggression arises in some situations but not in others.

41
Q

Evaluation of the frustration- aggression model to do with real-life application

A
  • studies have found that the presence of an aggressive cue increases aggression therefore can have real life application such a concealing guns in countries such as America
42
Q

Evaluation of of the social learning theory to do with it cannot explain all types of aggression

A

–there are two broad categories of aggression recognised by researchers: reactive (‘hot-blooded’, angry) and proactive (‘cold-blooded’, calculated). Children who are experienced in using proactiv aggression have high levels of self-efficacy - they are confident that their aggressive behaviour will bring benefits. They use aggression as a way of achieving their goals (i.e. instrumentally). This type of behaviour is well-explained by social learning theory.
However, reactively aggressive children habitually use aggression to retaliate in the heat of the moment. They tend to be hostile, suspicious of others, and do not use aggression to achieve anything except retribution. This behaviour is less explicable from a social learning perspective, and may be better explained by Leonard Berkowitz’s negative affect theory.
Therefore cannot explain all aggression( other model better explain…)

43
Q

Evaluation of the social learning theory in explaining aggression- media influence

A

-Rowell Huesmann and Leonard Eron
(2013) argue that media portrayals of aggressive behaviour can be powerful influences on a child’s acquisition of aggression. This is especially true if a character (in, for example, a soap opera) is rewarded for being aggressive, and if the child is able to identify with the character in some way (for instance, he or she has appealing traits, such as a sense of humour). Under these conditions, vicarious reinforcement experienced by children observing violent behaviour by media characters may be just as influential in encouraging imitation as it is in real life.
Should we be encouraging it to children?
Real-life application in avoiding aggressive children ?
Change the way media is portrayed?

44
Q

Evaluation of deindivuation in aggression to with evidence for it can lead to pro social behaviour aswell

A
  • Robert Johnson and Leslie Downing (1979) conducted a study where female participants had to give (fake) electric shocks to a confederate. In one condition, the participants were dressed in a Ku Klux Klan-type outfit, with masks hiding their faces. Participants in another condition dressed as nurses, and a third group (control) wore their own clothes. Compared with the control group, the KKK-dressed participants gave more (and more intense) electric shocks, and the ‘nurses’ gave fewer at lower levels. The researchers also noted that the nurses were more compassionate towards their ‘victim’, in line with the prosocial role associated with a nurse’s uniform. It seems that both aggression and prosocial behaviour are potential outcomes of de-individuation (not just aggression), and normative cues in the situation determine which is most likely to occur.
45
Q

Research into the importation model of aggression

A

DeLisi and his colleagues (2011) studied 813 juvenile delinquents confined in institutions in California. These were inmates who brought into confinement several negative dispositional features such as experiences of childhood trauma, high levels of anger and irritability, a history of substance abuse, and a history of violent behaviour.

Findings These inmates were more likely to engage in suicidal activity and sexual misconduct, and committed more acts of physical violence that were brought to the attention of the parole board (compared with a control group of inmates with fewer negative dispositional features).

46
Q

Evaluation of deprivation model =mixed evidence

A

-Cunningham et al. (2010) analysed 35 inmate homicides in Texas prisons and found that motivations for the behaviours were linked to some of the deprivations identified by Clemmer.
Particularly important were arguments over drugs, homosexual relationships and personal possessions.
As these are factors predicted by the model to make aggression more likely, these findings support its validity

However,
Contradictory research
The deprivation model predicts that a lack of freedom and heterosexual contact should lead to high levels of aggressive behaviour in prisons. However, the available evidence does not support this. For example, Christopher Hensley et al (2002) studied 256 male and female inmates of two prisons in Mississippi, a state of the USA which allows conjugal visits (that is, visits from partners specifically to have sex). There was no link between involvement in these visits and reduced aggressive behaviour. This shows that situational factors do not affect prison violence.

47
Q

Evaluation of the importation/deprivation model to do with importance of both

A

Shanhe Jiang and Marianne Fisher-Giorlando (2002) suggest that the importation model is a better explanation of violence between inmates, but the deprivation model is more useful in understanding innate aggression against prison staff. Rhonda Dobbs and Courtney Waid (2004) argue in favour of an interactionist model. Inmates entering prison for the first time will suffer deprivation. But deprivation does not necessarily lead to violence unless or until it combines with the individual characteristics imported into the prison by inmates, and which influence the prison’s culture.

48
Q

The effects of computer games in aggression- correlational studies

A

-Matt DeLisi et al. (2013) studied 227 juvenile offenders, all with histories of serious aggressive behaviours such as hitting a teacher or parent, or gang fighting. Using structured interviews, they gathered data on several measures of aggression and violent computer game-playing. They found that the offenders’ aggressive behaviour was significantly correlated with how often they played violent computer games and how much they enjoyed them.
The researchers argued that the link is so well-established that aggression should be considered a public health issue, like HIV/AIDS, and computer game violence a significant risk factor, like condom non-use.

49
Q

The effects of computer games on aggression-longitudinal studies

A

-Lindsay Robertson et al. (2013) wanted to see if there was a link between what they called
‘excessive’ television viewing in childhood and aggressive behaviour in adulthood. They studied 1037 people born in New Zealand in 1972 and 1973, and measured their TV viewing hours at regular intervals up to the age of 26 years. They found that time spent watching TV was a reliable predictor of aggressive behaviour in adulthood, measured in terms of convictions for aggressive and violent crimes. Those who watched the most TV were also more likely to be diagnosed with antisocial personality disorder and to have aggressive personality traits. It appears that the most important media-related factor in influencing aggressive behaviour may be the amount of TV watched rather than whether it has violent content or not.

50
Q

The effects of computer games-meta analysts

A

Craig Anderson et al. (2010) performed a meta-analysis of 136 studies which included all three types of methodology outlined above. They found that exposure to violent computer games was associated with increases in aggressive behaviours, thoughts, and feelings. This finding was true for both males and females and across collectivist and individualist cultures. Furthermore, the higher quality studies in the analysis showed an even greater significant effect. To put the outcome into perspective, the researchers claim that the effect of violent game-playing on aggressive behaviour is greater than the effect of second-hand smoke on cancer. Importantly, the analysis also showed no indication that publication bias influenced the results

51
Q

Research into the deprivation model

A
  • Steiner(2009) investigated the factors that predicted inmate aggression in 512 prisons in US.
  • found = inmate-on-inmates violence was more common in prisons were higher proportions of female staff,African-American inmates, Hispanic inmates and inmates in protective custody for their own safety. All the factors are independent of the individuals characteristics therefore supporting deprivation model