Week 5: Foraging behavior Flashcards
Foraging behavior
searching for and consuming food
Foraging behavior is a critical part of every animal’s existence
Many animals spends a good deal of their waking hours foraging
Anti-fungi mutualism
Ant-fungi mutualism is a symbiosis seen in certain ant and fungal species
The ants promote the growth of the fungi (good for the fungi), while also eating the vegetative shoots produced by their fungal partners (good for ants)
This mutualistic relationship began about 50 million years ago
Streptomyces bacteria
A whitish-grey crust found on and around many ants with fungus food gardens
A mass of Streptomyces bacteria - a type of bacteria that produces many antibiotics
Hypothesis: ants use the bacteria’s antibiotics to kill parasites that grow in their fungal gardens
Ants use antibiotics to regulate parasitic disease
All 20 species of the fungus-growing ants had Streptomyces bacteria associated with them.
Ants transmit the bacteria across generations.
When male and female reproductive ants are examined, only females possess the bacteria.
The bacteria found on fungus-growing ants produce antibiotics that wipe out only certain parasitic disease. Escovopsis, a serious parasitic threat to the ants’ fungus garden.
Starlings
Starlings fly from their nest to a feeding site (patch), search for a load of prey by probing in the grass, then take them home to the nestlings.
The bird is very efficient at probing. But when it has a load of leatherjackets already in its bill, it becomes less efficient. For this reason it is not necessarily the best thing to fully load its beak.
Starling’s problem of load size
traveling time: the amount of traveling back and forth from the nest to a foraging site
searching time: probing the prey in a foraging site and catching it
The first couple of leatherjackets are found quickly and easily, but because of the encumbrance of the prey in its beak, the bird takes longer and longer to find each successive prey.
Within each foraging trip, diminishing returns arise because, as it loads up with prey, the starling’s efficiency at collecting further prey is progressively diminished.
Loading curve
Load as a function of search time
The longer the starling has been foraging, the less likely it is to find another leatherjacket in a patch.
–> When should the starling give up on this curve?
Diminishing returns
- handling time
- depletion
- prey in the patch take evasive action and become harder to catch
- the predator becomes less likely to search new areas in the patch
- the predator starts with the easy prey and then goes on to hunt for those that are more difficult to catch
—> Loading curve is a curve of diminishing returns
Rate of intake
‘best” option = maximising net rate of delivery of food
rate of intake = load/ (traveling time + searching time)
Optimal foraging theory
max rate of intake = max load/(traveling time + searching time)
—> the slope of the line between A and the intersection with the gain curve
Traveling time
Suppose that the starling now switches to feeding at a close site with a short travel time, how should its load per trip change?
optimum is less for the shorter travel time.
Load size as a function of round trip time
Kacelnik (1984): tested the prediction of the model of load size
- trained parent starlings in the field to collect mealworms
- mealworms were dropped in a wooden tray
- varied the intervals of dropping mealworms
- the trained bird waited on the wooden tray for mealworms
the load size increased as a founding of round trip time
Marginal value theorem
Foraging in a patch:
- prey are distributed in patches
- prey within patches are depleted by foragers
- forager pays travel cost to reach other patch
What should a forager do to maximise the rate of gain?
- leave the patch when the rate of gain becomes marginal
- marginal value theorem (MVT)
Application of MVT:
- applicable to lots of situation in which an animal exploits a resource that occurs in discrete patches, and within a patch it experiences diminishing returns
Optimality theory
- the notion that adaptations have greater benefits-to-costs ratios than the putative alternatives that have been replaced by natural selection
- currency: the costs and benefits impinging on the animal
- constraints: mechanisms of behavior and the physiological limitations of the animal
3 advantages of optimality modeling:
- testability
- testable, quantitative predictions
- easy to tell whether hypotheses are right or wrong - explicit assumptions
- the bird encounter only one patch at a time, - generality
Bobwhite quail coveys
Northern bobwhite quail spend the winter months in small groups, called coveys, which ranges in size from 2 and 22 individuals.
Benefits: anti-predator defense; the larger the group is, the more safe. Then levels off around a group size of 10
Costs: Increased competition for food in larger groups, forcing more movement
Optimal covey size for northern bobwhite:
- Most are composed of intermediate size, with about 11 individuals.
- Daily survival rate are highest in coves of this group size.