Week 1 - phonological perception and discrimination Flashcards
Areas of language development
– Phonology: the study of sound as a linguistic system
– Prosody: the study of how our intonation affects language
– Lexicon: totality of the words in a language
– Morphemes: sounds combined to make units which are not full words e.g., ‘ment’ in ‘government’
– Morphology: morphemes are combined to make larger words, phrases or sentences e.g., ‘govern’ and ‘ment’ for ‘government’
– Syntax: the rules of language
– Pragmatics: social use of language
The “Cat in the Hat” study (DeCaspar and Spence, 1986)
- Mothers 7 ½ months pregnant read one story to their unborn babies
- Twice a day until birth (average of 67 times)
- Each mother read aloud 1 of 3 variations of the same story
- Story 1: first part of the original story
- Story 2: adaption – same rhythm, different words ‘The Dog in the Fog’
- Story 3: adaption – different rhythm, different words ‘The king, the mice and the cheese’
- Stories had similar length and shared 60-80% vocabulary
- At 3 days old the babies heard recordings of all three stories
- Strong preference shown for the story read to them before birth
Based on the findings DeCaspar and Spence believed that the babies were picking up prosodic aspects of their mothers’ speech (intonation) and could process, recall, and recognise these after birth.
The High Amplitude Sucking Paradigm (HAS Paradigm)
– Non-nutritive sucking: dummy connected to a computer via a tube – tube measures differences in pressure
– Pressure indicates sucking – changes in strength of pressure indicate changes in sucking rate
– The sucking rate is our dependent variable (what we are interested in measuring)
Rhythm-based language discrimination hypothesis
infants can discriminate between two foreign languages based on intonation
Vowel-based discrimination Hypothesis:
infants can discriminate between languages based on the rhythmic representation of the language:
Vowel-based discrimination Hypothesis:
infants can discriminate between languages based on the rhythmic representation of the language:
Evidence:
Infants can discriminate between:
• Stress-timed and syllable times languages (English/Catalan, English/Italian)
• Stress-timed vs mora-timed languages (English/Japanese)
Children do not discriminate languages within the same class e.g., English/Dutch
Bertoncini et al, 1998
Habituation a group of infants to a set of four syllables sharing the same vowel (bi, si, li, mi)
Habituation of another group of infants to a set of four syllables sharing the same consonant (bi ba, bo ,bƏ)
Experimental phase – same sequence plus a new syllable pairing:
• Either the vowel with the syllables used in the habituation phase (bi si li mi di)
• OR the consonant with the syllables used in the habituation phase (bi ba bo bƏ bu)
If the di/bu is perceived as a new stimulus infants sucking rate should increase
Findings:
• Infants detected the presence of a new syllable when the vowel of the new syllable differed e.g., bi ba bo bƏ bu
• Infants did not detect the new syllable when the consonant differed e.g., bi si li mi di
Werker and Tees (1984)
This indicates that we are born with the ability to differentiate between any sound contrasts even ones we have never heard before, but this ability is lost as we grow older.
Showed that 6–8-month-olds are ‘universal listeners’ as they can discriminate Hindi contrasts as well as Hindi-speaking adults.
Our ability to differentiate between phonemic contrasts outside of our native language diminishes at the end of the first year of life
But the ability to differentiate between phonemic contrasts within our native language is maintained at 100%
We become attuned to sound contrasts in our own language and our discrimination of native contrasts improves with age.
Pigdins and Creoles
- Pidgins – languages that are created from a mix of lexical items from one or more languages but with its own, primitive grammar.
- Hawaiian Pidgin English arose on sugarcane plantations in Hawaii during the early 20th century when immigrant workers from Japan, Korea, and the Philippines came together; they shared no language with one another (Bickerton, 1981, 1984)
- Creole: a language that used to be a Pidgin and subsequently became a native language for some speakers (Todd, 1974)
- Swahili may be the result of contact between Arabic and Bantu language (Todd, 1974)
• The Language Bioprogram Hypothesis (Bickerton, 1984)
humans are endowed with an innate skeletal grammar that constitutes part or all of the human species-specific capacity for syntax.
Methods of neurolinguistic investigation
- Lesion method – correlate bits of missing brain with bits of missing psychological functioning (Damasio, 1988)
- Studying split-brain patients provides a unique window into how each hemisphere functions
- Dichotic listening task – presenting information to the right and left side of the brain to see if the person reports hearing the information presented in the right ear if the stimuli is presented to the left hemisphere.
- Functional brain imaging methods – present stimuli to patients and obtain data on where the brain is most active as it processes those stimuli
- EEG/ERP – measures electrical activity in the brain via electrodes on the scalp. The location of ERPs associated with different mental activities is taken as a clue to the area of the brain responsible for those activities (Caplan, 1987).
- Optical topography – light emitting and light detecting devices are placed on the scalp at particular landmarks and measures of light transmission are taken as evidence of oxygenation of the blood and thus neural activity occurring between the emitter and the detector (Pena, Maki, Kovacic et al., 2003).
The equipotentiality hypothesis – at birth the left and right hemispheres have equal potential for acquiring language (Bishop, 1983)
• Evidence: The degree of asymmetry in brain function increases with development and the ability of the right hemisphere to take over language functions for a damaged left hemisphere is greater in children than in adults
– at birth, the left and right hemispheres have equal potential for acquiring language (Bishop, 1983)
• Evidence: The degree of asymmetry in brain function increases with development and the ability of the right hemisphere to take over language functions for a damaged left hemisphere is greater in children than in adults
Invariance hypothesis
– left hemisphere has the adult specialisation for language from birth and that lateralization does not change with development
• Evidence
• Neuroimaging studies of intact children suggest some cortical specialisation from birth
• Developmental changes in which hemisphere handles language may also arise from changes in how children process language as they gain expertise. For example, experienced musicians showed a right hemisphere advantage for music whereas naïve listeners showed a left ear advantage.
• Children who suffer early brain damage also subsequently experience seizures, so it is difficult to untangle the effects of the initial brain damage from the effects of the seizures (Rowe, Levine, Fisher and Goldin-Meadow, 2009)
The critical period hypothesis (Lenneberg, 1967)
- Lenneberg – language acquisition is an ‘age-limited potential’
- A biologically determined period during which language acquisition must occur
- Critical periods within other species e.g., imprinting in birds
- Some environmental input is necessary for normal development, but biology determines when the organism is responsive to that input