Video Module 17: Concepts and Categories Flashcards

1
Q

What are concepts and why do we use them to represent objects in our minds?

A

concepts are rich representations of things in the world
- concepts are interrelated to other concepts
- concepts help us form more general representations of objects; if we didn’t have them, we’d have to rely on only specific instances of things we encounter
- concepts assist our cognitive economy by allowing us to minimise processing effort and resources

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

How do concepts increase cognitive processing efficiency?

A

Concepts help us with memory, reasoning, communication, creating complex or new concepts from pre-existing ones stored in our minds, and generalising objects to larger categories.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

What are key aspects of concepts?

A
  1. typically stable
  2. related to other concepts
  3. typically a single word
  4. help us understand the world
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

How do concepts and categories differ?

A

Concepts are typically stable mental representations. Categories are ways of grouping items together on a basis of a certain criteria, and categories are not always stable.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

natural kinds

A

groupings or categories for items that occur naturally
- e.g. birds, trees, plants

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

artifacts

A

groupings or categories for man-made items or ideas
- e.g. appliances, furniture, liberty, justice

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

stable categories

A

those which people generally agree on what goes in them and what the criteria of inclusion are
- e.g. writing instruments, desserts, cats

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

ad hoc categories

A

those which are unstable and defined for a specific purpose within a specific context
- e.g. “people sitting in the front row of PSYC 105”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

basic level of categorisation (features)

A
  1. has its own word
  2. not too general, not too specific
  3. easy-to-explain commonalities between members
  4. people’s initial or instinctive response to categorising items (e.g. “chair” vs. “swivel chair”)
  5. tends to be used in speaking & reasoning about categories
  6. children learn basic level categories first
  7. not necessarily fixed; context and level of knowledge can alter what is a “basic level” category
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

superordinate categories

A

those which include the basic level category along with other basic level categories; those which are above basic level categories
- e.g. “furniture” is a superordinate category for “chair”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

subordinate categories

A

those which fall under the basic level category; those which are included in the basic level category
- e.g. “corgi” is a subordinate category of “dog”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

How does categorisation help us make inferences about the world?

A

Categories help us make inductive and deductive inferences.
We can reach a general conclusion about members of a category based on specific examples of members in the category (inductive reasoning). We can also reach a specific conclusion about a given instance based on the general principles of the category to which that instance belongs (deductive reasoning).
- categorisation is an inductive inference because we infer which categories objects fall into based on their features

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

What have researchers discovered in terms of the way that children make inferences based on categories?

A

A study by Gelman and Markman (1987) showed children a picture of a “bird” (flamingo) and a “bat,” telling them about the different ways they feed their babies. The researchers then asked children to make an inference about the way that a “bird” (a blackbird) feeds its babies.
- 85% of preschoolers in this condition guessed that the blackbird mashed up its food, like the flamingo
- The researchers found that children are sensitive to the labels that adults use for objects; they are easily able to learn new facts and extend them to other members of the same category

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

classical theory of categorisation

A

The theory that a category is defined in terms of necessary and sufficient features.
- necessary features: those which an object must possess
- sufficient: enough to place an object in a category
- abstract representation of objects as lists of features; suggests that we do not store any information of specific exemplars
- all-or-none membership: sth is either a member of a category or not

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

What are the criticisms of the classical theory of categorising objects?

A
  1. for many categories, there aren’t distinct/defining features: often you can remove any particular feature of an object and still have it belong to a category (e.g. a dog with no tail is still a dog)
  2. non-necessary features of objects can affect people’s categorisation choices: e.g. if a shape has all the requirements for a rectangle, people may still categorise it as a square if it has sides equal in length
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

family resemblance (categorisation)

A

a method of categorisation in which there is no defining feature that members of a category must have, but there tends to be characteristic features that are common or typical amongst members of the category

17
Q

graded membership

A

the likelihood that an object belongs to a certain family/category
- the idea that items can be ‘better’ or ‘worse’ members of a category
- category membership is a matter of degree: a member is more likely to be part of a family if it has a greater number of typical features
- suggests that categorisation is probabilistic rather than deterministic: that having certain features probably makes an object a member
- categorisation is a matter of similarity; category boundaries are fuzzy

18
Q

typicality effects

A

in which people tend to see some members of a category as “more typical” than others
- some differences between members of a category are related to non-necessary features

19
Q

How can we assess typicality effects?

A
  1. rating tasks: ppl rate the “X-ness” of an object
  2. production tasks: ppl produce/write down as many category members as they can think of in an allotted amount of time
  3. sentence verification tasks: ppl mark statements of objects as T or F
  4. generalisation tasks: ppl decide if generalisations can apply to whole categories based on specific instances
20
Q

rating tasks (typicality effects)

A

in which ppl rate the “X-ness” of an object
- e.g. “How bird-like is this bird?”
- Researchers find that typicality ratings may be based on the total number of typical features a category member has
—this may explain common errors of categorisation, for example when people group dolphins with fish

21
Q

generalisation tasks
(typicality effects)

A

in which ppl decide if generalisations can apply to whole categories based on specific instances
- e.g. “If penguins can catch this disease, can all birds?”
- researchers find that features of a typical member are more likely to be believed as applicable to all category members than are features of an atypical member

22
Q

sentence verification tasks
(typicality effects)

A

in which ppl mark statements of objects as T or F
- 3 kinds of statements: true (typical), true (atypical), and false
- E.g. “Fish is meat.” (true, atypical)
- researchers measure response time and find that RT is faster for more typical examples

23
Q

production tasks
(typicality effects)

A

in which ppl produce/write down as many category members as they can think of in an allotted amount of time
- researchers measure order and frequency of examples listed amongst participants
- more typical examples are produced/recalled first and more frequently

24
Q

What are challenges to the classical theory of categorisation?

A
  1. It’s nearly impossible to give adequate definitions of objects in a category
  2. You can remove particular features of objects can still have them belong to a category
  3. Typicality effects: people’s judgements of category membership reflect graded membership