Twin Design and Role of Environment Flashcards

1
Q

What is the twin design? (and citation)

A

The twin design disentangles the relative contributions of genes and environment, across a variety of human traits. The classical twin design is based on contrasting the trait resemblance of monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs. Monozygotic twins are genetically identical and derived from one fertilised egg (zygote), dizygotic twins develop from separately fertilised eggs and are 50% similar genetically (Polderman et al 2015).
If genetic factors are important for a trait, MZ individuals must be more similar than first-degree relatives or DZ twins.

Using data on twin similarity we decompose the total phenotypic variance of a trait into Additive genetic (A), shared environment (C), and non-shared environment (E), with both MZ and DZ having 100% shared environment and 0 non-shared. Twin data enable the different variance components to be estimated, because MZ and DZ twins have different degrees of correlation for the genetic components (A) but the same degrees of correlation for the environmental components (C and E)

most studies focus on same sex DZ as they provide a more appropriate comparison to MZ twins, who are always of the same sex.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

MZ may have more similar experiences but this is not a violation as differences are not caused environmentally CITE

and although some suggest MZ are treated more similarly CITE

this differential treatment does not significantly affect twin similarity for behaviours such as cognitive abilities (CITE),

A

Eaves et al 2003

Scarr 1968

Morris-Yates, et al, 1990,

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

what is heritability? how can we estimate it using twins? give example and cite

A
•	Heritability (h2) is an index for the relative contribution of genetic effects to the total phenotypic variance. It tells us about that trait, measured in that specific population, at that specific time, but doesn’t tell us about individual risk
The heritability (h2 ) of the phenotype can be estimated from twice the difference between MZ and DZ correlations
•	For example, typical MZ and DZ correlations for depression are about 0.4 and 0.2 (Kendler et al., 1992) and therefore heritability is estimated at ~40%
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Heritability and intelligence

A

There is substantial heritability for intelligence, with genetics typically accounting for 50% of the variability (Deary, 2013), although estimates have ranged from 30% to 80% (Deary et al., 2009). Longitudinal twin research has shown that heritability rises across the lifespan (Davis, Haworth & Plomin, 2009), increasing from 41% in childhood, to 66% in young adulthood (Haworth et al., 2010). In contrast, influences from the shared environment become less predominant with age, accounting for 74% of the variance in early childhood, and 33% in middle childhood (Davis et al., 2009). These changes in aetiological influences could explain the range of heritability estimates reported by Deary et al., (2009).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

casuality from twin design - 2 studies

A

In addition to confirming genetic and environment influences, twin studies can also be used to confirm relationships and causality by establishing whether they are due to genetic or environmental confounding. For example:
Stadler et al., (2012). - explored the association between subjective wellbeing and increased longevity using twin pair analyses. As expected, at the individual level (without regard to twin pair membership), SWB predicted increased longevity. Exposure effects were also present in unadjusted and adjusted within-pair analyses of dizygotic (DZ) pairs and monozygotic (MZ) pairs, indicating that SWB is associated with increased longevity independent of familial factors of genes and shared environment, consistent with a causal link between SWB and longer lifespan.

De Moor, et al., 2008 - tested causality in the association between regular exercise and symptoms of anxiety and depression. In genetically identical twin pairs, the twin who exercised more did not display fewer anxious and depressive symptoms than the co-twin who exercised less. = Regular exercise is associated with reduced anxious and depressive symptoms in the population at large, but the association is not because of causal effects of exercise.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

another assumption of twin design is that results can be generalised to the rest of the population. Specifically, the twin method assumes that twins are similar to singletons. There are many ways in which twins have been found to differ from singletons (CITE) eg
birth weight, lower IQ than singletons, however.. ?

A

(Evans and Martin, 2000);

those studies that have found differences between twins and singletons have been conducted on young twins, and studies on older twins confirm that these differences have all but disappeared by early to middle childhood

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

SEM - evaluative point from..

A

• STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELLING (SEM), is an approach in which genotypic and environmental effects are modelled as the contribution of unmeasured (latent) variables to the potentially multivariate phenotypic differences between individuals. The latent factors represent the effects of many unidentified influences.

Bulik et al., 2000 – in their review argue that SEM is quite flexible and rather complex models can be easily specified. It provides a scientifically rigorous framework that allows previously articulated hypotheses to be evaluated by a clearly specified set of statistical rules and competing hypotheses can be directly compared. SEM can be depicted graphically in the form of a path diagram which is convenient and often clarifying.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Haworth, Kovas, Dale & Plomin 2008- cholesky

A

investigated the etiology of academic performance in Science and its etiological links with other academic abilities and general cognitive ability (g). = Our multivariate results show that Science shares genetic influences with English, Math and g. Nonetheless, Science is more than just g, as there are specific genetic and environmental influences on Science. In addition, there are common environmental influences shared by the three school subjects that are not shared with g — a school environment effect.
These findings were revealed using a Cholesky decomposition model which allowed for measurement of the unique and common genetic and environmental influences.
=In this case, the first factor assesses genetic, shared and non-shared environmental influences on g, some of which may also influence English, Math and Science. The second factor estimates influences on English, some of which may also influence Math and Science, and the final factor estimates influences that are unique to Science.
=genetics unqiue to academic perfomance, and science
=shared e specific to g

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Haworth et al., 2013

A

Multivariate twin analyses were used to investigate the genetic and environmental links between environment and outcome. This study used data from the Twins’ Early Development Study (TEDS; Oliver & Plomin, 2007).

To assess the science-learning environment we used items from the Classroom, Home and Peer Environment Influences Scale. A test was used to examine scientific enquiry skills, these are skills needed to design and evaluate scientific evidence, and are a key component in the UK National Curriculum.

To investigate the links between the learning environment and science performance, we conducted bivariate twin model-fitting. Bivariate model-fitting decomposes the covariance between traits, providing estimates of the genetic and environmental correlations between traits.

= The most surprising result was that the science-learning environment was almost as heritable (43%) as performance on the science test (50%), and showed negligible shared environmental influence (3%). This science learning environment included separate subscales of the classroom environment and the peer environment which were strikingly similar.
Bivariate twin analyses indicated that 56% of the phenotypic correlation between the science-learning environment and science performance was explained by genetic influences, indicating gene–environment correlation.

BUT
However, note that because the phenotypic correlation is only 0.225, this means that overlapping genetic factors explain just a small proportion (2.8%) of the total variance in science performance. There are at least two explanations for the low correlation - ?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

TEDS evaluative point

A

The TEDS sample has been shown to be reasonably representative of the general population in terms of parental education, ethnicity and employment status (Kovas et al., 2007

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Haworth 2008

A

= genetic influences account for over 60% of the variance in scientific achievement, with environmental influences accounting for the remaining variance. Environmental influences were mainly of the non-shared variety, suggesting that children from the same family experience school environments differently.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Tucker-Drob and Harden (2012)

A

provided evidence for gene-environment correlations using longitudinal data from identical and fraternal twin pairs.
• The first set of analyses tested the directionality of the association between parenting and early cognitive ability, using two regression models. The first controlled for cognitive stimulation at 2 years and found that children’s cognitive ability at 2 predicted the quality of stimulation received from their parents at 4 years.
• The second regression model tested the reciprocal association (scores at 2 years predicting parenting behavior at 4 years, controlling for baseline parenting behavior).
• Next, data from both twins in each pair was used to estimate a series of behavioral genetic models. Following the conventions of the classical twin model, variance in each phenotype was decomposed into three components: ACE.
• The associations between the phenotypes were modeled using a Cholesky decomposition, in which each subsequent phenotype is regressed onto the A, C, and E components of all preceding phenotypes
= phenotypic associations between parental cognitive stimulation and child cognitive ability were reciprocal. Notably, the standardized regression coefficients from phenotypic models were approximately equal, indicating that children’s abilities predict their parents’ future behavior as strongly as parents’ behaviors predict their children’s future abilities.
=Parenting influenced cognitive development through an environmental pathway, whereas children’s cognitive ability influenced subsequent parenting through a genetic pathway. These results suggest that genetic influences on cognitive development occur through a transactional process, in which genetic predispositions influence early cognitive development leading children to evoke stimulation of differing levels of quality from their parents, and these early levels of stimulation by parents act as effectual environments in boosting their children’s subsequent cognitive development.

HOWEVER, the data analyzed were limited in some respects. First, parenting and cognition data were only available for a relatively narrow period during early childhood: ages 2 years to 4 years. More longitudinal measurements over an extended age range would be useful to examine how gene-environment transactions unfold over the entire span of child development.
ALSO, while the current twin design was informative about the operation of children’s genes, it was insufficient for making inferences about the operation of parents’ genes. That is, while we found that parenting affected cognitive development through a family-level environmental pathway, our design was not capable of determining the extent to which parenting behaviors were themselves influenced by parents’ genes

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

MZ twins may have more similar experiences than DZ twins because they are more similar genetically. Such differences between MZ and DZ in experience are not a violation to the equal environment assumption because … (CITE)

A

the differences are not caused environmentally (Eaves et al., 2003).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

twins tend to have lower birth weights and are at greater risk of perinatal complications than singletons (CITE)

In childhood, language develops slower in twins, and twins perform less well on tests of verbal ability and IQ (CITE). These delays are similar for MZ and DZ and tend to be a result of postnatal environment rather than pre-maturity (CITE).
However, most of the deficits are recovered in early school years (CITE) and twins do not appear importantly different from singletons for cog abilities (CITE) personality (CITE) or psychopathology (CITE).

In addition, most studies have not matched twins with singletons in terms of genetic background nor early environmental experiences. A recent study comparing twins with their siblings (who are matched in both genetic background and early environmental experiences) in Dutch WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) scores, failed to detect any differences between twins and their siblings in cognitive abilities (CITE)

A
O’Brien et al., 1987
Ronald et al., 2005
Rutter et al, 1991
Christensen et al., 2006
Johnson et al 2002
Robbers et al., 2011
Reiss, et al, 2000.
Posthuma et al., 2000
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

are twins generalisable?

Ronald et al., 2005
Rutter et al, 1991
Christensen et al., 2006
Johnson et al 2002
Robbers et al., 2011
Reiss, et al, 2000.
Posthuma et al., 2000
A

In childhood, language develops slower in twins, and twins perform less well on tests of verbal ability and IQ (Ronald et a., 2005). These delays are similar for MZ and DZ and tend to be a result of postnatal environment rather than pre-maturity (Rutter et al., 1991).
However, most of the deficits are recovered in early school years (Christensen et al., 2006) and twins do not appear importantly different from singletons for personality (Johnson et al 2002) or psychopathology (Robbers et al., 2011).
A host of studies comparing older twins with singletons have failed to find differences in physical characteristics and cognitive abilities and psychological outcomes (Reiss, et al, 2000.)

In addition, most studies have not matched twins with singletons in terms of genetic background nor early environmental experiences. A recent study comparing twins with their siblings (who are matched in both genetic background and early environmental experiences) in Dutch WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) scores, failed to detect any differences between twins and their siblings in cognitive abilities (Posthuma et al., 2000)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

haworth 2013 - percentages, phenotypic correlation and overall variance explained

A

science-learning environment was almost as heritable (43%) as performance on the science test (50%), and showed negligible shared environmental influence (3%).

the phenotypic correlation is only 0.225, this means that overlapping genetic factors explain just a small proportion (2.8%) of the total variance in science performance.

17
Q

haworth 2009

A

In 2007, the Genetics of High Cognitive Abilities (GHCA) Consortium was formed with the goal of combining cognitive ability test scores from six twin studies in four countries (USA, UK, Australia and the Netherlands) to identify sufficient numbers of twins with high g scores to conduct adequately powered analyses of the genetic and environmental etiology of high g

Using data from the (GHCA) Consortium, Haworth et al., (2009) investigated the genetic and environmental etiologies of high general cognitive ability (g). For these analyses we classified high performance as scores above the 85th percentile (in each study).

= Genetic influence for high g was substantial (0.50, with a 95% confidence interval of 0.41 – 0.60). Shared environmental influences were moderate (0.28, 0.19 – 0.37). They conclude that genetic variation contributes substantially to high g
= The authors previously reported that results for the normal distribution of g were similar for the entire GHCA sample of 11,000 twin pairs: Heritability was estimated as 0.55 (0.51 – 0.59) and shared environment was 0.21 (0.17 – 0.25) (Haworth et al., 2009). The overlapping confidence intervals suggest that the etiology of high g is not significantly different from the origins of individual differences in g throughout the normal distribution. BUT, the large confidence intervals for high g suggest caution in concluding that there are no differences in the etiology of high g and the normal distribution of g. Moreover, similar heritabilities do not necessarily imply that the same genes affect high g and the normal distribution of g.

=BUT the estimates of shared environment for g in twin studies are greater than estimates from family and adoption sibling designs. It is reasonable to assume that because twins are the same age and grow up in the same family at the same time, they share their experiences to a greater extent than other siblings. Future research on the genetics of high g could estimate the extent of a special twin shared environmental effect by including non-twin siblings.

Finding a high heritability for intelligence does not mean it cannot be changed. Heritability describes the extent to which individual differences in g can be attributed to genetic differences between individuals given the genetic and environmental differences that exist in a particular population at a particular time. Therefore heritability of g could be 100% but environmental interventions could improve performance on tests that assess g

18
Q

what is multivariate twin analysis?

A

Multivariate twin analysis focuses on the covariance (correlation) between two or more traits. It uses the twin method to estimate genetic and environmental contributions to their covariance as well as the
variance of each trait
Multivariate analyses are based on cross-trait/cross-twin
correlations — e.g., Math in twin 1 correlated with English in
twin 2.

19
Q

Structural equation model-fitting allows …

A

more complex analyses, formal tests of significance, and the calculation of confidence intervals

20
Q

what is the generalist genes hypothesis?

A

One of the most important recent findings from quantitative genetic research such as twin studies is that the same set of genes is largely responsible for genetic influence across these domains. We call these “generalist genes” to highlight their pervasive influence. In other words, most genes found to be associated with a particular learning ability or disability (such as reading) will also be associated with other learning abilities and disabilities (such as mathematics).

21
Q

the heritability of longevity - cite name

A

Finkel et al., 2014

22
Q

Life events have an estimated heritability of 28% (CITE)

A

Kendler and Baker, 2007

23
Q

Plomin et al., 2007 - three conclusions drawn from their study

A

We draw three conclusions that go beyond estimating heritability.
First, the abnormal is normal: Low performance is the quantitative extreme of the same genetic and environmental influences that operate throughout the normal distribution.
Second, continuity is genetic and change is environmental: Longitudinal analyses suggest that age-to-age stability is primarily mediated genetically, whereas the environment contributes to change from age to age.
Third, genes are generalists and environments are specialists: Multivariate analyses indicate that genes largely contribute to similarity in performance within and between the three domains—and with general cognitive ability—whereas the environment contributes to differences in performance.

These conclusions have far-reaching implications for education and child development as well as for molecular genetics and neuroscience.

24
Q

Describe the cholesky model and when it is most suited and why it is good

A

The Cholesky procedure is similar to hierarchical regression analyses in non-genetic studies, where the independent contribution of a predictor variable is assessed after accounting for its shared variance with other predictor variables.

The Cholesky model is most suited to variables that can be ordered, as was the case for the longitudinal analyses presented in the previous chapter in which the variables can be ordered by age. An important feature of the Cholesky model is that it can be used to estimate genetic variance shared by English and mathematics that is independent of “g.”

25
Q

describe narrow verse broad-sense heritability

A

Narrow-sense heritability (h2) refers only to the additive genetic component (A) and represents the degree to which genes transmitted by parents determine the phenotype of their children. Broad sense heritability (H2) is the ratio of total genetic variance (A and D) divided by the total phenotypic variance. It includes nonadditive genetic variance and thus includes gene x gene interactions and dominant effect.

Studies use the Dominant genetic (D) variance model when the MZ correlations are double that of DZ.

26
Q

why may narrow sense estimates be inflated?

A

if family resemblance is influenced by non-additive genetic effects (dominance or gene–gene interaction), shared familial environments, and by correlations or interactions among genotypes and environment.