SPH528 - Week 2 Paediatric Speech Flashcards

Goal Setting Risk Factors Consonant Acquisition

1
Q

Harrison and McLeod, (2010). Risk and protective factors associated with speech and language impairment in a nationally representative sample of 4-5 year old children
- Nine factors consistently identified as having a unique effect on speech and language development

A
  • being male (risk)
  • having ongoing hearing problems (risk),
  • having a more reactive temperament (risk),
  • having a more persistent temperament (protective),
  • having a more social temperament (protective),
  • increased maternal well-being (protective),
  • having an older sibling (risk/protective),
  • parental LOTE status (risk/protective), and
  • support for children’s learning in the home (risk/protective).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Sensitivity is the proportion of children with SSD
identified correctly.
-What is specificity

A

Specificity is the proportion of typically developing children not identified as having SSD.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

This type of Ax for children with SSD gathers qualitative descriptions of success and difficulties in participation in the family, school and social situations. Assesses ICF domains of activity and participation.

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Intelligibility Assessment

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Some contextual factors to consider when goal setting for children with SSD:

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Parent/carer, family and school involvement, support, attitudes and expectations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Personal factors to consider when goal setting for a child with SSD:

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Age of the child, self-confidence, motivation, attention, cognitive ability, beliefs, learning style, and how they deal with (or prior experiences with?) communication failure during intervention.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

What is the most knowledge method (also called the traditional, or developmental approach)

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

An approach to setting goals for children with SSD whereby sounds or phonological processes are selected as intervention goals because they are earlier developing, stimulable and (already) produced correctly in particular contextual environments (Bernthal and Bankson, 1998).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

What is the least knowledge method (nontraditional, nondevelopmental approach)

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

An approach to setting goals for children with SSD whereby Treatment targets differ from the child’s existing abilities by multiple features’ (Elbert and Gierut, 1986). Sounds or processes are selected because they are nonstimulable, phonetically more complex, have phonologically marked properties, reflect least phonological knowledge as categorized by inventory constraints (with respect to phonemes) and are later acquired (Gierut et al., 1996).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

ICF model specifies consideration of activities and participation in facilitating health and well-being. Some activities and participation are:

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Speaking; conversation; learning and applying knowledge; interpersonal interactions and relationships; community, social and civic life.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

The influence of an SSD on learning and applying knowledge is part of the consideration of ICF activities and participation. What implications does this have for goal setting?

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Intervention targeting phonological awareness should accompany speech intervention (Gillon, 2000). Partnerships b/n teachers and SLPs essential for appropriate goal setting to facilitate oral and literate communicative skills.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

How can SLPs consider participation within speaking and conversation by children with speech impairment, when planning for intervention?
(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

Consider interpersonal interactions and relationships -social assessment practices may consist of a discussion with the child, his/her parents, siblings, friends, grandparents and other significant people to ascertain social goals and barriers, interests and hopes (Duchan, 2001a).
Consider community, social and civic life  socially based intervention goals to facilitate activity and participation (e.g., Goal: public education to decrease background noise and encourage people to look at a person with hearing impairment while talking to them).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

ICF allows for comparison between performance and capacity when considering activity and participation. How is this useful?

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

A child may be more capable within the confines of a standardized assessment than they demonstrate within the complex communicative demands of the classroom. OR a child may use compensatory strategies to minimize communicative difficulty, but the difficulty is evident on standardized assessment tasks. Mismatch between performance and capacity. –> needs to be considered when developing appropriate goals.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

ICF Environmental factors: products and technology; support and relationships; attitudes; services, systems, and policies. How do these impact appropriate goal setting for a child with SSD?

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A
  • Barriers/facilitators avoided/ available to be utilised need to be considered in goal setting (e.g., parental support and relationships)
  • Negative judgments about people with SSD  goals directed at society may be needed.
  • Products and technology e.g., future voice-activated technologies may be inaccessible.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

This ICF Personal factor is important to consider when selecting speech goals for a child with SSD, and can be improved. Perhaps incorporated into goals?

(McLeod and Bleile, 2004)

A

The child’s personal ability to evaluate their speech / self-evaluation prior to feedback from the SLP.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Name 6 approaches to target selection for children with SSD.

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A
Traditional developmental
Complexity
Cycles
Systemic (functional)
Constraint-based nonlinear
Neuro-network
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Name 3 perspectives on identifying goals for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Impairment
Social
Biopsychosocial (i.e., ICF-CY (WHO, 2007))

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q
  • Improving a child’s speech intelligibility
  • Surgical repair of a cleft palate
  • Developing a child’s emergent literacy skills to ameliorate future risk of literacy difficulties
  • developing a child and communication partner’s competence to use AAC
  • Increasing a child’s verbal interaction with peers at preschool/school
  • Reducing immediate consequences of SSD such as bullying
A

*Equipping families with strategies for resolving communication breakdowns
*Increasing family members’ and relevant professionals’ knowledge about SSD to dispel false beliefs and myths
*Improving communication partners’ abilities to listen to a child with SSD so that the child’s messages are understood.
-> Goals are not limited to the child who has the SSD - they can include others in the child’s life.
(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Some example of goals for a child with SSD, that address contextual factors:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A
  • Child’s school to develop and anti-bullying program to change the attitudes of peers.
  • For the SLP to lobby relevant governing bodies about the need for SLP services in rural areas, so the child in question gets services.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Goals must be OPERATIONALLY DEFINED to be measurable. This means include the following (specified, so as to be measurable).

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A
  • Behaviour or attitude to be learned (e.g., intelligible speech during conversation)
  • Task used to measure skill/behaviour/attitude (e.g., conversational speech sample.)
  • WHO will conduct the measurement?
  • SETTING where the measurement will take place
  • Criterion (e.g., with 90% accuracy from a conversational sample containing at least 100 different words
  • Expected duration of intervention (e.g., within 12 months of regular weekly SLP intervention combined with daily home practice).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

HOW TO IDENTIFY AND PRIORITISE AN INDIVIDUAL CHILD’S GOALS:

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A

Careful consideration of: Child’s Ax results (ALL ICF domains) + Research evidence + Child and family preferences

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Goals related to BODY STRUCTURE for a child with SSD of known origin) are likely to be…

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Medical intervention (e.g., eliminate repeated episodes of glue ear - ENT to prescribe antibiotics, myringotomy, pressure equalisation tube…)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

How to Ax a child’s relevant Contextual Factors (Environmental & Personal factors)?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A
  • Case Hx, intake form, interview with teacher, interview with siblings/friends…
  • > SOME CONTEXTUAL FACTORS CAN BE TRANSLATED INTO GOALS. SOME MIGHT JUST BE IDENTIFIED AS BARRIERS AND FACILITATORS AND WORKED WITH OR WORKED AROUND WHEN DEVELOPING THE INTERVENTION PLAN.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

How to Ax a child’s relevant Activities and Participation?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A
  • information gathered from child and their family during Ax (ie case Hx, SPAA-C)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

3-level goal hierarchy (Klein & Moses, 1999)

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A
  1. Long-term goals  (typically) summarise what needs to be achieved before discharge.
  2. Short-term goals  specific skills/behaviour targeted, to achieve long-term goal.
  3. Session goals  SHORT-TERM GOALS are transformed into SESSION GOALS (behaviours, skills, or knowledge taught through intervention procedures) within an activity during intervention sessions with an intervention agent
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

What do session goals typically specify? (8)

NOTE: Session goals vary from one session to the next, depending on a child’s progress

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A
  • Child’s observable behaviour (e.g., production of multiple opposition treatment word sets including word-initial /d/ contrast with word-initial /k, ʃ, sl, tʃ/ at word level).
  • Response mode (e.g., with a model for delayed imitation),
  • Response level (e.g., at word level, phrase level),
  • Teaching and learning procedure(s) (e.g., metaphor, auditory models of treatment words for delayed imitation),
  • Dose (e.g., two treatment sets or approximately 40 trials),
  • Criterion (e.g., 70% accuracy),
  • Intervention agent (e.g., the clinician, computer), and
  • Context (e.g., drill- or play-based activities in the clinic).
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

What does the hierarchical organisation of goals usually mean?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Session goals contribute to short-term goals, and short-term goals contribute to the realisation of a long-term goal.
If the realisation of one goal facilitates realisation of a different goal, without direct intervention on that goal GENERALISATION is happening.

26
Q

What is Generalisation (transfer)?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A

Desired change in a behaviour in an intervention context facilitates change in the same behaviour and/or different but related behaviours in other non-intervention contexts (Gordon-Brannan & Weiss, 2007; Stokes & Baer, 1997).

27
Q

Generalisation is one of the most important outcomes of intervention for a child with SSD (i.e., generalised acquisition of the correct production of a targeted phoneme during everyday conversational speech means intervention to target that phoneme in other contexts is not needed). So the SLP should plan for generalisation from…?

A

…from the outset of intervention - at the point where goals are being identified and prioritised.

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

28
Q

What are the 2 types of generalisation?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A
  1. Stimulus generalisation

2. Response generalisation

29
Q

What is stimulus generalisation?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A
  • a trained behaviour is evoked with a different stimuli (Bernthal, Bankson, & Flipsen, 2013). Example: A child saying affricate /tʃ/ in ‘chicken’ when an SLP says “What’s this?” and presents the child with a picture of a chicken, in the clinic  Child using this affricate in the word ‘chicken’ in response to seeing a picture in a book read by his dad, when his dad asks “What kind of animal lives on this farm?”
30
Q

What is response generalisation?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A

-The process in which responses carry over to other behaviours that are not taught (Bernthal, Bankson & Flipsen, 2013, p. 255).
Examples: (articulation impairment) targeted speech skill improves across untreated word positions, in untreated words during conversational speech.
(phonological impairment) increase in a child’s productive phonological knowledge generalisation to other words/word positions NOT TARGETED during intervention. E.g., child taught to say /tʃ/ in ‘chicken’ uses /tʃ/ in cheese without being taught

31
Q

Most children with SSD have more than one issue requiring intervention. How does an SLP prioritise goals to work on, in which order, or whether goals can be worked on concurrently?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Consider research evidence + family preferences
**With phonological impairment, WHAT IS TREATED MAY BE MORE IMPORTANT THAN HOW IT IS TAUGHT (Gierut, 2005, p. 203). Because some intervention targets can induce more widespread and efficient change in children’s phonological systems.

32
Q

Name 6 target selection approaches reported in the literature since Elbert and McReynolds (1979) work on generalisation:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A
  1. Traditional developmental approach
  2. Complexity approach
  3. Cyclical approach
  4. Systemic approach
  5. Nonlinear approach
  6. Neuro-network approach
33
Q

What is the traditional developmental approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD? (Most knowledge)
-> Lower hanging fruit - may be helpful for a child with a reactive temperament or fear of failure. May be helpful to use these targets if a child is getting frustrated by therapy due to lack of success.

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Developmental norms regarding age of acquisition of speech sounds apply. Appropriate targets include phonological processes that:
• occur frequently but are optional (i.e., targets that the child has some phonological knowledge of)
• affect sounds that are stimulable or within a child’s phonetic inventory
• affect intelligibility, such as idiosyncratic processes or extensive harmony
• affect early developing sounds

34
Q

What is the complexity approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD? (Least knowledge)

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Set complex intervention targets, beyond or outside their existing knowledge as it will facilitate learning of the phonological system they’re trying to learn  Evidence that it leads to greater generalisation, so fewer targets (and therefore short-term goals) are needed to achieve the long-term goal.

35
Q

What is meant by COMPLEXITY and LEARNABILITY, when using the complexity approach to setting intervention targets in a child with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

COMPLEXITY - fricatives in a phonological system imply the presence of plosives (but plosives don’t imply the presence of fricatives)  Fricatives are more complex than plosives.
LEARNABILITY - What a child knows about a phonological system is a subset of what an accomplished speaker of the language knows…

36
Q

What are complex intervention targets?

McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10

A

Non-stimulable, phonetically more complex segments or CCs associated with least productive phonological knowledge for an individual child (consistently in error). Marked, later developing targets.

37
Q

Which children with SSD are suited to the complexity approach?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Most (maybe all) children with SSD. Some believe better suited to confident risk-takers with no complicating difficulties (ie small syllable structure, word length, stress inventories, atypical receptive language etc), cognitive or attention limitations.

38
Q

What is the cycles approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Patterns (or processes) evident in children’s speech are identified then targeted in a predetermined order, with primary target patterns being selected prior to secondary target patterns, which are in turn selected prior to advanced target patterns.
Criteria to move onto another target are not performance-based, but instead time-based, with most patterns being targeted for 2-6 hours (Hodson, 2007).

39
Q

What is the Systemic (Functional) approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

A specific sound is not targeted, rather the phonological function of a carefully selected group of sound is targeted. AIM: To reduce the homonymy of pronouncing so many different words the same, due to big collapses of contrast

40
Q

What are the intervention targets chosen, when following the systemic (functional) approach to target selection for intervention in children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. Ch 10)

A

Targets for intervention are chosen to be maximally distinct from the phoneme the child is using in place of a large number of phonemes not in their inventory. For example if a child is collapsing multiple phonemes into [d], a target that is maximally distinct in MOA, POA, voicing, CC vs C, from [d] would be chosen.

41
Q

What is the Constraint-Based Nonlinear Approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 360)

A

Instead of targeting sounds for intervention both content (speech sounds) and frame (word and syllable structures, timing units, stress patterns) are considered for target selection. Nonlinear theories are used to guide analysis of a child’s speech.

42
Q

What is the Neuro-Network Approach to target selection for intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 362)

A

Instead of targeting specific sounds/sound classes, more general goals are selected, i.e., increase a child’s PCC in conversational speech, add six new phonemes to a child’s phonemic inventory EXCEPT in the case of distortions of /s, ɹ/ (Norris & Hoffman, 2005). Based on Norris & Hoffman’s (2005) constellation model of language processing, where 9 different levels of speech processing interact with each other to assimilate and accommodate new input in a self-organisation of the neural network.

43
Q

How do I know how to choose the best target selection approach for an individual child?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 363)

A

No easy answer. Integrate peer-reviewed published evidence with child characteristics and preferences. Consider the child’s personal and social contexts, language, cognitive, perceptual, and motor abilities when prioritising goals (Bernhardt, 2005).

44
Q

Goals for children with articulation impairment typically involve:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 363)

A
  1. target sounds (rhotics or sibilants produced incorrectly)
  2. activities and participation
    ->If more than one sound is in error select the most stimulable, correct in more contexts, earlier developing, more motivating for child to practice, more impact on intelligibility.
    Also consider family preference and impact on child’s activities and participation.
45
Q

Intervention goals for children with inconsistent speech disorder is to:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 364)

A

…Establish lexical consistency. Target will be consistent production of a core vocabulary of words (selected by the child/family in collaboration with SLP and the child’s teacher)
-> Additional goals addressing activities and participation and contextual factors on a case-by-case basis.

46
Q

Goal setting for children with CAS depends a number of factors:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 364)

A

Child’s age, nature and severity of the impairment, presence of concomitant conditions, and motivation. May be a broad range of goals addressing intelligibility in addition to activity and participation.

47
Q

A generic “pro forma” for a long-term goal for a child with phonological impairment or CAS -> Don’t use - just to help me understand the ‘format’ of this kind of goal.

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 364)

A

The child will develop functional and intelligible communication based on a period of conversation with specified communication partners in specified contexts in a designated period of time, as measured by a specified functional communication outcome measurement tool

48
Q

What to work on with a child with CAS might be influenced by what the child needs to be able to say, to participate in their day-to-day. Rather than individual sounds, they need to practice…

A

…movement transitions, in the context of speech and to develop appropriate intonation and prosody.

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 367)

49
Q

Some common short-term goals for children with childhood dysarthria who have the potential to use spoken communication include…

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 367)

A

■■ improve articulation accuracy of problematic consonants and/or vowels, syllable shapes, or word lengths;
■■ encourage respiratory support and breath control for speech, therefore improving speech intelligibility;
■■ reduce hypernasality; and
■■ increase the naturalness of a child’s phonation through increasing a child’s awareness of and use of appropriate pitch and vocal loudness.

50
Q

Potential goals for children with highly unintelligible speech:

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. pp. 367-8)

A

Socially based goals may need to be prioritised to get the child communicating and reduce their frustration ie AAC, targeting a small vocab of meaningful words, teaching others in their environment to understand the child’s productions.
GOALS generated by the family with SLP support.

51
Q

What is a GOAL ATTACK STRATEGY in intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 368)

A

The way in which multiples goals are scheduled.
3 contrasting goal attack strategies (Fey, 1986).
1. Vertical (training deep)
2. Horizontal (training wide)
3. Cyclical

52
Q

What is a VERTICAL goal attack strategy in intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 368)

A

Targeting of 1-2 speech targets (e.g., phonemes, phonological processes, word shapes, stress patterns, prosodic pattern) at a time until a predetermined performance criterion
is met. RECOMMENDED for children with articulation impairment with 1-2 distorted speech sounds or children with a phonological impairment. Multiple opportunities to learn 1-2 targets over a period of time, before new targets added

53
Q

What is a HORIZONTAL goal attack strategy in intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 368)

A

Working on several (3+) speech production targets PER SESSION. Helps children with phonological impairment see the relationship between the targets in the phonological system they’re learning. For children with MOTOR SPEECH difficulties, horizontal attack strategy may enhance motor learning.

54
Q

What is a CYCLICAL goal attack strategy in intervention for children with SSD?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 368)

A

Working on several speech production targets within a specified amount of time, regardless of accuracy. One speech production target or phonological process is targeted per session, for a certain number of hours. No predetermined performance criterion before moving on to the next one, just predetermined amount of time.

55
Q

What are 3 goal attack strategies for children with concomitant SSD and language impairment?

(McLeod & Baker, 2017. p. 368)

A
  1. Block scheduling - e.g., 10 weeks of SSD intervention followed by 10 weeks of language intervention.
  2. Alternating sequencing - e.g., SSD intervention on even weeks and language intervention on odd weeks.
  3. Simultaneous scheduling - e.g., SSD intervention and language intervention conducted within the same session.
56
Q

List some factors that indicate risk of speech and language impairment:

Harrison & McLeod (2010)

A

Being male
Ongoing hearing problems
Having a more reactive temperament
Having an older sibling (protective for receptive language, risk for expressive)
Parental LOTE status (Risk for standardised Ax, protective for parental concerns about language).

57
Q

Plosives, nasals, nonpulmonic consonants are produced earlier than…

McLeod and Crowe (2018)

A

trills, flaps, fricatives, affricates

generally, across 27 languages studied

58
Q

MOST labial (bilabial and labiodental), pharyngeal (pharyngeal, epiglottal, and glottal), and posterior lingual (palatal, velar, and uvular) consonants are produced EARLIER than consonants with anterior tongue placement (dental, alveolar, postalveolar, and retroflex).EXCEPT…

A

…plosives and nasals with anterior tongue placement (e.g., /t, d, n/)
Which are produced EARLIER than anterior trills (e.g., /r/), liquids (e.g., /l/), fricatives (e.g., /s, z, ʃ, ʒ/), and affricates (e.g., /ʧ, ʤ/).

(McLeod and Crowe, 2018. p.1554)

59
Q

Most consonants across world languages are acquired by 5;0 (93% produced correctly). True or false?

A

True

McLeod and Crowe (2018)

60
Q

ENGLISH
Early (2;0–3;11): /p, b, m, d, n, h, t, k, ɡ, w, ŋ, f, j/
Middle (4;0–4;11): /l, ʤ, ʧ, s, v, ʃ, z/
Late?

McLeod and Crowe (2018)

A

Late (5;0–6;11): /ɹ, ʒ, ð, θ/