Psychology Studies Flashcards
Kluver & Bucy
They destroyed the amygdala in a dominant monkey causing it to lose its dominant place in the social group. Monkeys failed to recognise the ‘emotional importance of events’.
Boccardi et al
habitually violent offenders exhibited abnormal hippocampal functioning
Scerbo & Raine
correlational analysis found consistently lower levels of serotonin in people described as aggressive
Mann et al
gave 35 healthy participants dexfenfluramine (depletes serotonin) and found an increase in hostility and aggression scores on a questionnaire.
(his findings only applied to males not females)
Wagner et al
aggression levels in mice decreased following castration, and then increased when the mice were later injected with testosterone
Dabbs et al
measured salivary testosterone in violent and non-violent criminals. Those with the highest levels had a history of violent crimes
Pardini et al
- conducted a longitudinal study whereby participants with varying histories of violence were subjected to MRI scans.
- the participants with lower amygdala volume exhibited higher levels of aggression and violence, even when confounding variables were controlled
Rosado et al
- compared a sample of dogs that had been referred due to their aggressive behaviour towards humans, with a control sample that did not exhibit aggressive behaviour
- the aggressive dogs had significantly lower serotonin levels than the non-aggressive dogs
Albert et al
positive correlations have been found between levels of testosterone and self-reported levels of aggression among prison inmates
Coccaro et al
- studied adult male twins:
- For direct physical assault: 50% concordance rates for MZ twins and 19% for DZ twins
- For verbal aggression: 28% concordance rates for MZ twins and 7% for DZ twins.
Hutchings & Mednick
- studied over 14,000 adopted males in Denmark
- found that the adoptee was most likely to become a criminal if both their biological and adoptive parents were criminals.
- biological parents (particularly fathers) had a greater influence on criminality.
Brunner et al
- studied 28 male members of a large Dutch family, who were repeatedly involved in aggressive criminal behaviours, e.g. rape and assault.
- these men were found to have abnormally low levels of MAOA
Caspi et al
- boys with the MAOA-L variant were significantly more likely to grow up to exhibit anti-social behaviour, but only if they had been maltreated as children
- boys with the MAOA-H variant who were maltreated and those with MAOA-L who were not maltreated did not display anti-social behaviour.
Stuart et al
-Men with MAOA-L =most violent perpetrators of IPV
Miles and Carey
- conducted a meta-analysis by combining the results of 24 twin and adoption studies
- they concluded that aggressive anti-social behaviour was largely a product of genetic contributions
- they also found that the influence of genes increased with age
Niehoff
-suggested that the reason why the MAOA gene may explain uneven violence rates between males and females, may be a consequence of the differential genetic vulnerability that males and females have to the MAOA gene, as it is linked to the X chromosome.
Lorenz
- suggested that the main function of aggression is adaptive as it is beneficial to survival= a ‘defeated’ animal is rarely killed, but gets territory elsewhere
- He also noted that little actual physical damage was done during fights= most aggressive encounters consist mainly of ritualistic signalling, e.g. displaying claws and teeth.
- This allows contestants to assess relative strength of opponent before fighting
Tinbergen
- He presented sticklebacks with a series of wooden models of different shapes
- Found that regardless of shape, if the model had a red spot (sign stimulus) the stickleback would display an aggressive FAP and attack it
- if there was no red spot, there was no aggression, even if the model looked more realistic
- FAPs were stereotyped, universal and ballistic.
Lehrman
-suggested that Lorenz had underestimated the role of environmental factors in the development of aggression
Chagnon
- He describes how, among the Yanomamö people of South America, chest pounding and lcub fighting contests can settle a conflict short of more extreme violence
- male warriors in traditional societies have more sexual partners and children
- aggression in combat can also increase status and so strengthen bonds between in-group males.
Goodall
- observed chimpanzees
- male chimps from one community slaughtered all the members of another group in a co-ordinated and premeditated fashion
- The violence continued despite the fact that the victims were offering signals of appeasement and defencelessness
- these signals did not inhibit the aggressive behaviour of the attacking chimps
Wilson and Daly
- They identified strategies used to prevent mate infidelity:
- Direct guarding: vigilance over a partner’s behaviour, restricting autonomy e.g. checking who they have been seeing, coming home early, installing tracking apps on their phone etc.
- Negative inducements: issuing threats or consequences for infidelity, e.g. “I’ll kill myself if you leave me”.
Wilson et al
- found that women who reported mate retention strategies in their partners were twice as likely to have suffered physical violence at the hands of their partners
- of these women, 73% required medical attention and 53% said they feared for their lives.
Dobash & Dobash
-studies of battered women have shown that in the majority of cases, women cite extreme jealousy on the part of their husbands/boyfriends as the key cause of violence directed towards them
Livingstone Smith
- human warfare is adaptive for obtaining valuable resources, mates and forging intragroup bonds
- displays of aggressiveness and bravery are attractive to females.
Shackelford et al
-found a positive correlation between men’s reports of their mate retention behaviours and women’s reports of their partners’ physical violence