PSYCH EXAM 3 Flashcards
Describe the connections between the different forms of memory (sensory memory, short-term
memory, long-term memory) and the concepts of sensation, perception, attention,
consciousness, and learning
How the Different Types of Memory Connect to Other Concepts:
Sensory Memory
-Sensation: This memory holds raw data from our senses (like sights and sounds) for a very short time.
-Perception: Helps make sense of sensory data (e.g., recognizing a face).
-Attention: If we focus on sensory input, it moves to short-term memory.
-Consciousness: We aren’t aware of most sensory memory because it’s so brief.
-Learning: It’s the first step where new information can enter the system to be learned.
Short-Term Memory (STM)
-Attention: We only keep in STM what we pay attention to.
-Consciousness: STM holds information we’re actively thinking about.
-Learning: STM organizes information so it can be stored long-term.
Long-Term Memory (LTM)
-Learning: Stores knowledge and skills we can use later.
-Perception and Attention: What we already know helps us understand and focus on new things.
Explain the method and results of Sperling’s “Whole Report” and “Partial Report” conditions.
Sperling’s Experiment
-Whole Report Condition:
Participants were shown a grid of letters (e.g., “XMRJ, PNKP, TRZF”) for a very short time.
They were asked to report as many letters as they could remember.
-Results: Participants could only recall about 4 letters, even though they felt like they had briefly seen all of them.
Interpretation: The sensory register holds more information than participants can report before it fades.
-Partial Report Condition:
After showing the same grid of letters, a tone signaled which row participants should recall.
-Results: Participants could recall about 3 letters from any row cued, showing the sensory register contains all the information, but only a portion can be moved into short-term memory (STM).
Describe what type of information (sensations) is kept in the sensory register.
-The raw sensory data from all senses, such as visual images, sounds, or tactile sensations.
Visual data (e.g., shapes, colors, patterns).
Auditory data (e.g., sounds, speech).
Tactile data (e.g., pressure, texture).
Describe the amount of information that is in the sensory register.
-The sensory register has a very large capacity, capturing most of the sensory input we experience at a given moment.
- while it sure has a large capacity, it can only keep stuff in for less than a second. So very briefly.
Describe how long information lasts in the sensory register
Sensory memory fades quickly:
-Visual sensory memory (iconic memory): Lasts less than 1 second.
-Auditory sensory memory (echoic memory): Lasts up to 2–3 seconds.
-When the delay between the stimulus and the cue exceeds 1 second, recall drops sharply.
Describe how we move information from the sensory register to short-term memory
Attention is the key process that transfers information from the sensory register to STM. Without attention, sensory information fades and is not encoded.
Describe why we typically aren’t aware of the existence of our sensory register
-It operates unconsciously and updates constantly.
-Information in the sensory register is rapidly replaced by new sensory input, so we rarely notice its existence.
Describe what the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad are
What are the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad?
Phonological Loop:
This is your “inner voice” that processes verbal and auditory information.
For example, repeating a phone number to yourself to remember it.
Visuospatial Sketchpad:
This is your “mind’s eye” that handles visual and spatial information.
For example, imagining a map or the layout of a room.
Describe how short-term memory errors show us that the information we store in short-term memory is typically verbal (not visual).
-Short-term memory errors often involve confusing items that sound alike (e.g., mistaking “B” for “P”) rather than items that look alike.
-This suggests that STM primarily stores information in a verbal or auditory form instead of a visual form.
Describe how long information lasts in short-term memory and explain the role of rehearsal in prolonging information in short-term memory.
-Duration: Without rehearsal, information in STM disappears in less than 2 seconds.
-Rehearsal: Actively thinking about or repeating the information keeps it in STM longer and allows it to be encoded into long-term memory.
Describe the amount of information that we can keep in short-term memory.
-Short-term memory can hold 7 ± 2 items (5 to 9 items).
-The exact amount depends on how efficiently the information is rehearsed and how it is grouped (chunked).
Explain what chunking is, and how chunking can increase the amount of information in short-term memory.
-Chunking: Grouping smaller bits of information into larger, meaningful units.
*Example: Instead of remembering “25121975” as 8 separate digits, you can chunk it into “25/12/1975” (Christmas Day).
-Why it works: Chunking reduces the number of individual items you need to remember, making it easier to rehearse and recall.
How chunking increases the amount of info in STM
-Chunking reduces the number of items to rehearse, making STM more efficient and increasing its effective capacity.
Describe what is meant by deep processing and shallow processing. Explain how deep
processing (or elaboration) can help us encode information into long-term memory.
-Deep Processing: Involves thinking about the meaning of information and making connections to what you already know.
Example: Thinking about whether “mouse” is an animal.
-Shallow Processing: Focuses on surface-level features like appearance or sound.
Example: Thinking about whether “mouse” rhymes with “house.”
Explain how deep
processing (or elaboration) can help us encode information into long-term memory.
Deep Processing:
Engages with the material meaningfully by analyzing, connecting, or interpreting it.
This helps create stronger associations, making the information easier to recall later.
Elaboration:
Adding context or connecting new information to prior knowledge reinforces memory storage.
Explain why diversification and distributed practice help us make new long-term memories.
Diversification:
Thinking about material in different ways creates multiple connections, making recall easier.
Example: Using examples, mnemonics, or comparisons while studying.
Distributed Practice:
Spreading out study sessions allows your brain to process and consolidate information over time.
Why it works:
Repeated exposure strengthens memory pathways.
Practicing at intervals forces retrieval, enhancing retention.
Explain how the concepts of deep and shallow processing might be used to explain why there is some evidence that taking notes by hand leads to better long-term memory encoding than taking notes on a laptop.
Laptop Notes:
Often involve word-for-word transcription, which is shallow processing.
You’re focused on typing quickly without engaging with the material meaningfully.
Handwritten Notes:
Require summarizing, paraphrasing, or creating concept maps, which is deep processing.
You must think about the material’s meaning to condense it into key points.
How do mnemonics help us remember information?
How do mnemonics help us remember information?
Mnemonics create meaningful connections between unrelated pieces of information.
Example: Using “SuperMan Helps EveryOne” to remember the Great Lakes (Superior, Michigan, Huron, Erie, Ontario).
They work by:
Providing memory cues (e.g., the first letters of a mnemonic).
Turning abstract or unrelated items into a coherent, memorable pattern.
Why is massed practice (“cramming”) less effective than distributed practice?
Massed Practice:
Involves repeating information in a short time.
Leads to shallow processing and poor long-term retention.
Distributed Practice:
Spreads study sessions over time, forcing retrieval and deeper engagement with the material.
Re-exposure helps you think about the material in new ways, reinforcing memory pathways.
Describe what is known as the encoding-retrieval match.
Encoding-Retrieval Match:
This concept suggests that the environment in which you learn information acts as a cue that can aid in retrieving that information later.
Example: You might recall information better if you are in the same environment where you originally learned it (e.g., studying in the same room where you will take the test).
Explain how retrieval cues can help us retrieve long-term memories (even ones that we didn’t necessarily know we had).
Retrieval Cues:
These are external stimuli, such as words, images, or environmental factors, that help trigger memories.
Example: In Tulving and Pearlstone’s (1966) study, participants who were given cues (like categories) were able to retrieve more words from memory, even those they didn’t recall initially without cues.
Benefit: Cues can help bring up memories that we didn’t actively recall before, as they serve as hints that activate the memory pathways associated with the original experience.
Describe the concepts of retroactive interference and proactive interference.
Retroactive Interference:
Definition: New information interferes with the ability to recall old information.
Example: Learning a new language makes it harder to remember vocabulary from a previous language.
Proactive Interference:
Definition: Old information interferes with the ability to recall new information.
Example: Calling your new partner by your old partner’s name.
Explain the concept of reconstructive remembering and describe how our memory schemas can influence what information we remember
Reconstructive Remembering:
Definition: This is the idea that memory is not a perfect recording of events, but rather a reconstruction influenced by our beliefs, expectations, and prior knowledge.
Schemas:
These are mental frameworks or organized scripts we use to make sense of the world.
Example: A typical “office schema” might include items like desks, chairs, and computers. If we are asked to recall the contents of an office, we might misremember or fill in missing details based on what we expect to see in an office.
Effect on Memory: Our schemas can cause us to remember things that fit our expectations or misremember details that don’t align with our schemas (e.g., thinking a book was in the room even if it wasn’t, because we expect to see books in an office).
Explain the difference between retrograde amnesia and anterograde amnesia.
Retrograde Amnesia:
In retrograde amnesia, memory loss occurs for events that happened before the injury or illness.
Example: A person might not remember things from their past, like childhood memories or details of their life before an accident.
Anterograde Amnesia:
In anterograde amnesia, the ability to form new memories is impaired after the injury or illness.
Example: A person cannot remember new facts, events, or experiences that occur after the accident, but they can remember their past.
Describe what abilities people with anterograde amnesia do, and do not, have.
Abilities Retained:
Implicit Memory:
People with anterograde amnesia may still have implicit memory, which includes skills and conditioned responses.
Example: They may still know how to ride a bike or play an instrument, even though they can’t recall learning these things.
Abilities Lost:
Explicit Memory:
They cannot form new explicit memories, such as facts, names, or details of events they encounter after the injury.
Example: They might meet someone, but after a short time, they cannot remember the person’s name or the conversation.
Short-Term Memory:
They may also struggle with short-term memory, such as remembering a list of items or events that happen within a short time frame.