Precedence Flashcards

1
Q

(Litovsky et al., 1999).

A

The ‘‘precedence effect’’ refers to a group of phenomena that are thought to be involved in resolving competition for perception and localization between a direct sound and a reflection (Litovsky et al., 1999).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Haas (1951)

Blauert (1997)

A

This effect has also been called the “Haas effect” after Haas (1951) and the “law of the first waveform” (Blauert, 1997).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Wallach et al. (1949)

A

The term ‘‘precedence effect’’ was originally coined by Wallach et al. (1949) in their classic study to describe the dominance of the lead stimulus characteristics in the determination of the spatial location of the fused image localization dominance.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

(Zurek, 1980; Perrott et al., 1989)

A

The precedence effect is only shown for sounds of discontinuous or transient character and is reflected in the finding that the ability to detect shifts in the location of the lagging sound (the echo) is reduced for a short time following the onset of the leading sound (Zurek, 1980; Perrott et al., 1989).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Blauert 1997

A

If the interval between the arrival of the two sounds is 1ms or less, the precedence effect does not operate; instead an average or compromise location is heard. This is called “summing localization” (Blauert, 1997).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Brandewie and Zahorik (2010)

A

The precedence effect plays an important role in our perception of everyday sounds. It enables us to locate, interpret, and identify sounds in spite of wide variations in the acoustical conditions in which we hear them. Without it, listening and understanding speech in reverberant rooms would be an extremely confusing experience (e.g. Brandewie and Zahorik 2010).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

(Brown, Stecker, & Tollin, 2014)

A

Precedence effect-like phenomena have been reported in nearly every organism examined, from amphibians to canines to humans; the premises and interpretations of these many studies have often focused on the utility of precedence effect for echo suppression, that is, avoidance of the perceptual effects of echoes that might otherwise confound sound localization in real environments. This view, in turn, holds that the precedence effect facilitates (depending on the organism) communication, environmental awareness, prey capture, and/or predator avoidance in natural environments (Brown, Stecker, & Tollin, 2014).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Zurek (1980)

A

Zurek (1980) suggested that the precedence effect is useful for the avoidance of inter-aural ambiguities.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

(Brown et al., 2015)

A

The biological basis of the precedence effect remains unclear, despite decades of study (Brown et al., 2015).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Bianchi, Verhulst, and Dau (2013)

A

Bianchi, Verhulst, and Dau (2013) investigated the contribution of cochlear peripheral processing and found evidence of lag suppression occuring at the basilar membrane in the time range from 1 to 4 milliseconds evidenced by the suppression of lag click-evoked otoacoustic emissions.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

(Fitzpatrick et al. 1999; Litovsky et al. 1999)

A

Beyond the cochlea, extensive reviews of neurophysiological precedence effect literature have considered the temporal extent of precedence effect-like neural responses at multiple levels of the auditory system—from auditory nerve to cortex (Fitzpatrick et al. 1999; Litovsky et al. 1999). These reviews collectively implicate the inferior colliculus as the site at which strong suppression of the lag is first observed on a time course consistent with psychophysical observations.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Xia et al. (2010)

A

Similarly, Xia et al. (2010) concluded that location-dependent suppression of in the inferior colliculus neurons can explain the precedence effect

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Brown et al. (2015)
(Bianchi et al., 2013)
Brown et al. (2015)

A

Brown et al. (2015) evaluated the precedence effect in bilateral cochlear implant users. The data suggest that aspects of the precedence effect can be elicited in cochlear implant users, who lack functional cochlea, thus suggesting that neural mechanisms are sufficient to produce the precedence effect. While cochlear-mechanics may still contribute to the precedence effect at low frequencies, data from Brown et al. (2015) suggest that the cochlea is not necessary for the precedence effect, at least over a range of higher frequencies

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

(Cranford, Andres, Piatz, & Reissig, 1993; Cranford & Romereim, 1992)

A

Binaural temporal processing ability assessed by the precedence effect measured in the sound field declines with age (Cranford, Andres, Piatz, & Reissig, 1993; Cranford & Romereim, 1992).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Abel et al. (2000)

A

Abel et al. (2000) observed a systematic decline in horizontal plane localization with increasing age, starting as early as age 30.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

(e.g., Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002; Roberts et al. 2002, 2003; Roberts and Lister 2004; Lister and Roberts 2005; Akeroyd and Guy 2011).

A

Likewise, a number of investigations over the past two decades have assessed the precedence effect in people with hearing loss (e.g., Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002; Roberts et al. 2002, 2003; Roberts and Lister 2004; Lister and Roberts 2005; Akeroyd and Guy 2011).

17
Q

(Roberts et al. 2002; Roberts and Lister 2004, 2004)

Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002; Akeroyd and Guy 2011

A

While data have been widely variable, hearing-impaired individuals clearly experience both fusion (e.g., Roberts et al. 2002; Roberts and Lister 2004, 2004) and localization dominance (Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002; Akeroyd and Guy 2011).

18
Q

(Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002)

A

However, the effect of localization dominance does appear to be weaker in hearing-impaired listeners than in normal-hearing individuals (Cranford et al. 1993; Goverts et al. 2002).

19
Q

Akeroyd and Guy (2011)

A

Research by Akeroyd and Guy (2011) demonstrated a significant correlation between localization dominance abilities and degree hearing loss, such that better hearing listeners demonstrated better localization dominance. One hypothesis for decreased performance of hearing-impaired subjects on binaural and spatial hearing tasks is that the binaural signal processing itself is less accurate in this population.

20
Q

Goverts, Houtgast, and van Beek (2001)

A

Goverts, Houtgast, and van Beek (2001) investigated the effect of mild symmetrical sensorineural hearing loss on the precedence effect. Using dichotic signals presented by headphone, stimulus onset dominance (the precedence effect for lateralization) was examined for six subjects. Data from subjects with mild sensorineural hearing loss show large amounts of variance and an overall decrease in the precedence effect, which cannot be explained on basis of reduced audibility.

21
Q

(Houtgast and Steeneken, 1973)

A

Reverberation, like noise, acts like a low-pass filter, in that it smooths the modulations in the stimulus envelope (Houtgast and Steeneken, 1973).

22
Q

(Helfer & Huntley, 1991).

A

However, reverberation is a more complex example of distortion because it not only causes masking (from reflected energy), but it also smears the temporal structure of the signal (Helfer & Huntley, 1991).

23
Q

(Helfer & Huntley, 1991)

Lutman et al., 1991; Price & Simon, 1984

A

In a study aimed to examine the influence of aging and hearing loss on consonant perception in reverberation and noise, at least some of the data suggest that hearing loss of varying degrees adversely affects older listener’s ability to identify distorted consonants (Helfer & Huntley, 1991); however, hearing loss was an inadequate explanation for all of the speech perception difficulties of older adults, consistent with other studies, suggesting that age-related deterioration in adverse listening environments may be independent of hearing status (Lutman et al., 1991; Price & Simon, 1984).

24
Q

Picou, Moore, and Ricketts (2017)

A

In a study aimed to evaluate the effects of hearing aid directional processing on subjective and objective listening effort, Picou, Moore, and Ricketts (2017) concluded that directional microphone technology in hearing aids has the potential to improve listening effort in moderately reverberant environments, but not in environments of low reverberation.

25
Q

Reinhart, Zahorik, and Souza (2017)

A

Similarly, Reinhart, Zahorik, and Souza (2017) varied the amount of degradation from reverberation to hearing aid input signals for hearing-impaired listeners completing a low-context sentence comprehension task and found that the benefit of digital noise reduction processing in hearing aids may depend on the amount of reverberation in the environment.

26
Q

Litovsky et al. 2006; Grantham et al. 2008

A

While cochlear implants can sometimes restore hearing through electrical stimulation, performance in “binaural” tasks, including sound localization, remains poor in most bilateral CI users (e.g., Litovsky et al. 2006; Grantham et al. 2008).

27
Q

(Brown, Stecker, & Tollin, 2014)

A

A certain constraint on performance is that the devices do not preserve temporal information carried by the input signal (e.g., most devices provide no temporal fine structure cues and are not synchronized across the ears), which particularly limits sensitivity to ITD (Brown, Stecker, & Tollin, 2014).

28
Q

(Kerber and Seeber 2013)

A

Poorer ITD sensitivity within individual subjects is associated with worse localization in reverberation (Kerber and Seeber 2013), suggesting in turn that the precedence effect, which strongly depends on ITD cues, may be abnormal in users of bilateral CIs.

29
Q

(Seeber and Hafter 2008)

Agrawal 2008

A

Both widely variable fusion echo thresholds (Seeber and Hafter 2008) and relatively weak localization dominance and lag suppression (Agrawal 2008) have been documented for cochlear implant users. In contrast, under direct stimulation, a procedure by which the binaural information provided by bilateral CIs can be precisely controlled via custom research processors, the precedence effect appears to be more intact.

30
Q
van Hoesel (2007) and Agrawal (2008)
(Agrawal 2008; Litovsky et al. 2010)
A

Data reported by van Hoesel (2007) and Agrawal (2008) suggested qualitatively similar discrimination suppression in bilateral CI and normal-hearing listeners, at least among bilateral CI users with adult-onset deafness (Agrawal 2008; Litovsky et al. 2010).