Phonology Flashcards

1
Q

Psychological reality of the phoneme allophone distinction

A

Sapir 1933 found that speakers of Southern Paiute (which has a lenition rule after vowels) realised certain sounds differently if asked to articulate the first and second syllable of a word.

Cowan & Levitt studied a woman’s language game, and find that she is not reversing letters but rather phonemes - silent letters ignored, homographs pronounced differently, diphthongs and affricates preserved as units.

Loan Phonology - Korean word-final [t(no audible release] is an allophone of phoneme /s/. Loan words with word-final post-vocalic /t/ are invariably assigned the /s/ phoneme (Martin 1992), which becomes evident when morphological endings are added to the word and the t is realised as an s because it’s no longer word-final.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Phoneme Theory

A

Phoneme theory posits that sounds for languages are stored as abstract units (phonemes) in the mind. They have underlying representations, but also ‘allophones’: phonetic variants of the underlying form which occur in specific contexts.

There are various views as to what the phoneme consists of (acoustic sound, articulatory gesture, feature matrix)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Exemplar Theory

A

According to exemplar theory, mental storage of sounds is not abstract, but a collection of memories of phones (instances of a sound’s use), words, and multi-word utterances. These can be used to categorise new sounds encountered.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

DEFINITION OF: Phoneme

A
  • form in which a sound is stored in the abstract mental grammar
  • form underlying a set of surface allophones
  • minimal unit of language that suffices to distinguish meaning
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

DEFINITION OF: Allophone

A
  • member of a set of speech sounds identified by a native speaker as the same sound
  • a non-contrastive surface manifestation of a phoneme
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Complementary Distribution

A

When 2 sounds occur in non-overlapping phonetic environments
Phonetically similar phones in complementary distribution are usually allophones

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Free Variation

A

When 2 or more allophones freely occur in the same phonetic environment with no semantic effect

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Minimal Pair

A

2 words that differ only by a single sound in the same position and have different meanings

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

HINTS FOR SOLVING PHONOLOGY PROBLEMS

A

1) Look for minimal pairs
2) List environments in which sounds occur (complementary distribution?)
3) are sounds in CD phonetically similar?
4) determine which form is the ‘elsewhere’ form
5) for the form conditioned by a rule, determine the rule: should describe a common process like assimilation in terms of natural classes of sounds interacting with neighbouring segments / syllable structure

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Features

A

Saussure thought humans divided the perceptual world into a binary opposites (+/-). This led to the proposal of ‘distinctive features’: phonetic features which distinguish phonemes. The idea is that a phoneme is composed of (strong view) / categorised by (weak view), a set of phonetic features (+/-).
As such, phonemes sharing features would form natural classes.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Evidence that natural classes influence linguistic processes.

A
Coronal Consonants (made with front section of tongue). 
In AmE yods are dropped after coronal consonants (glide suppression) but not labials or velars.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Evidence for features: Synchronic Alternations

A

the allophonic relation between voiced stops and fricatives (intervocalic) in spanish can be described as the spread of [+continuant] feature to [+voice, -sonorant] segments].

English place assimilation for can be described as the spread of the place of articulation (coronal, dorsal, labial) feature leftwards from the [-nasal] segment to the [+nasal] segment.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Evidence for features: Historical Change

A

Grimm’s Law: Innovation where [-voice] segments become [+continuant]

ease of articulation?

u could say synchronic alternations are results of historical changes which happened due to ease of articulation requirements (e.g final devoicing from difficulty voicing without a following vowel), don’t need to be encoded in features - but for the weak view, this still works, and psycholinguistic evidence!

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Psycholinguistic evidence for features

A

Language disorders: stopping (+continuant to -continuant), devoicing (+voice>-voice)
could say it’s just muscle control failing
consonant

Consonant Vowel Dissociations
Dysgraphic patients show a strong tendency for substitution errors (82%) and transposition errors (62%) to preserve the C/V status of letters. (percentages for one patient)
Cotelli et al 2003 patient only struggled writing vowels (selective dysgraphia)
Caramazza, Miceli, Chialant 2000 - one patient made errors 3 times as often on vowels, other same pattern for consonants

Mismatch fields and Voice
pa ta ka da pa ka ta
(only the voiced one detected as different by Broca’s area - this is the only feature which appears in a many to one ratio)
(Phillips, Pellathy & Marantz 2000)

Speech Errors: Fromkin 1971
[voice] glear plue sky
[place] computation > pɒŋkjuːteɪʃn̩
[nasal] cedars of lebanon > cedars of lemadon

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Evidence for features from FLA

A

STOPPING: p for f [maip] ‘knife’
! t for s, θ [tuwt] ‘suit’; [ba:t] ‘bath’
! d for z, ð [dʌd] ‘does’; [ʌdʌ] ‘other’

Place harmony:
dorsal harmony - take → [gek]
labial harmony - zeep ‘soap’ → [fep] (dutch)
coronalisation/velar fronting - kiss > tiss

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Different positions on how sounds ‘phonemes’ are stored in the brain

A
  • traditional PHONEME theory - an abstract underlying acoustic form
  • Motor theory - a gestural score
  • features (strong view) - feature matrices
    ^these ones (abstract theories) have top down and bottom up processing
  • Exemplar theories - memories of words and multiword sequences are stored
17
Q

Evidence for exemplar based theories

A

(See Lent 1 essay for actual figures and reference)

French liaison - can’t be accounted for by simple before vowel rules bc in fact percentage liaison is correlated to frequency

thought to be an irregular variant so preserved in frequent elements

18
Q

What is the cohort model?

A

Marslen-Wilson’s cohort model is the idea that items in the lexicon are activated segment by segment during speech processing.

if a stimulus is presented up to or beyond the recognition point (the point where only one word contains that initial sequence of phonemes), only one word is primed, whereas if the recognition point of the stimulus is not reached, multiple words – the cohort – are primed. For example, in Pienie Zwitserlood’s study of Dutch ‘Kapit’ (the initial segments of both ‘Kapitein’ and ‘Kapitaal’), words relating to both words were primed, but using ‘Kapitein’, only words relating to ‘Kapitein’ were primed.

19
Q

Traxler notes that there is no 1-1 relationship between acoustic signal and phoneme. Is this a problem?

A

bottom up processing could involve a matching process (most similar phoneme), discarding unnecessary phonetic information.

20
Q

Evidence for the Motor Theory

A

This is evidenced by the McGurk Effect: if someone sees somebody’s lips saying ‘ga’, but the sound produced (in edited footage) is ‘ba’, the listener might hear ‘da’. This shows that when it is available, we use visual information related to production to perceive speech, implying that our understanding of speech sounds is related to their production rather than only simply the acoustic signal. In addition, nNeuroimaging data shows that there is a lot of activity in the motor cortex when listening to speech (Pulvermuller et al), and sounds produced in different places correspond to activity in different parts of the motor cortex.

However, there is also evidence to the contrary: Perception can occur by those who cannot produce speech. According to Eimas et al (1971) - Children are fully capable of perceiving the differences between many different speech sounds despite being incapable of producing them. In addition, Japanese Quail and Chinchillas are capable of perceiving speech sounds of phoneme size (learning to respond to some speech sounds and refrain from responding to others, and showing evidence of compensation for coarticulation) but they obviously do not have a human-like articulatory apparatus. So although motor cortex activity and the McGurk effect suggest a link between perception and production, the fundamental process of speech perception cannot rely on the ability to produce speech as shown by the cases of children and animals.

21
Q

Problem with proposing that phonemes ARE feature matrices.

A

doesn’t account for length

22
Q

The idea behind phonological rules

A

that we extract predictive generalisations rather than responding mechanically to statistical data.

This suggests you use rules (feature based) to map phonetic plan from underlying to surface form, rather than memorising all input data (you couldn’t deal with new loan words) or even using analogy (what if words ended with novel phonemes).

Take the pigeon study - they extracted the simplest rule possible, despite there being additional important data.

23
Q

What does + mean in rules

A

morpheme boundary