P3: Cognition and Development. Flashcards

1
Q

Paper 3 - Option 2: Cognition and development. The specification.

A

Cognition and development: subject content.

Cognition and development

Piaget’s theory of cognitive development: schemas, assimilation, accommodation, equilibration, stages of intellectual development. Characteristics of these stages, including object permanence, conservation, egocentrism and class inclusion.

Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development, including the zone of proximal development and scaffolding.

Baillargeon’s explanation of early infant abilities, including knowledge of the physical world; violation of expectation research.

The development of social cognition: Selman’s levels of perspective-taking; theory of mind, including theory of mind as an explanation for autism; the Sally-Anne study. The role of the mirror neuron system in social cognition.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Describe the basic assumptions of the cognitive approach.

A

Basic Assumptions

Mediational processes occur between stimulus and response:

Behaviorists rejected the idea of studying the mind because internal mental processes cannot be observed and objectively measured.
However, cognitive psychologists regard it as essential to look at the mental processes of an organism and how these influence behavior.
Instead of the simple stimulus-response links proposed by Behaviorism, the mediational processes of the organism are important to understand. Without this understanding, psychologists cannot have a complete understanding of behavior.

Psychology should be seen as a science:

Cognitive psychologists follow the example of the behaviorists in preferring objective, controlled, scientific methods for investigating behavior.
They use the results of their investigations as the basis for making inferences about mental processes.

Humans are information processors:

Information processing in humans resembles that in computers, and is based on based on transforming information, storing information and retrieving information from memory.
Information processing models of cognitive processes such as memory and attention assume that mental processes follow a clear sequence.
For example:

Input processes are concerned with the analysis of the stimuli.

Storage processes cover everything that happens to stimuli internally in the brain and can include coding and manipulation of the stimuli.

Output processes are responsible for preparing an appropriate response to a stimulus.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Describe cognitive development and Piaget’s theory.

A

What Is Cognitive Development

Children grow and develop rapidly in their first five years across the four main areas of development. These areas are motor (physical), language and communication, cognitive and social/emotional.

Cognitive development means how children think, explore and figure things out. It is the development of knowledge, skills, problem solving and dispositions, which help children to think about and understand the world around them. Brain development is part of cognitive development.

As a parent, it is important to foster your child’s cognitive development as soon as he/she is born because doing so provides the foundation for your child’s success in school and later in life. For example, research shows that children who can distinguish sounds at six months of age are better at acquiring the skills for learning to read at four and five years of age.

Piaget: Contribution & basis:

Jean Piaget (1896–1980) is considered by many to be the most influential psychologist to study human cognitive development. Indeed, the structure of the UK education system is largely based on his findings.

Prior to Piaget, children were viewed as smaller versions of adults. Their thinking was seen as different in quantity but it was not fully appreciated that it was thequalityof their thinking that mattered most. For example, you will recall the learning theory (behaviourist) approach which suggests that our learning is the sum of stimulus response connections.

Piaget’s investigations demonstratedhowchildren’s thinking varied from adults rather than howmuchit varied. The characteristics described by Piaget were influential in the development of education systems across Europe, not only in the age bands for different types of schools but also in the methodology used by educational practitioners to teach children.

Belief and conclusions on the theory:

Piaget believed that cognitive development results from the interaction of innate abilities and environmental events. He also thought that knowledge is actively discovered and that intelligence exists as a state of equilibrium, when the individual is able to cope with thedatathey are assimilating. Intelligence is therefore not static but continually changes by adapting itself to new environmental stimuli.

Piaget was originally a biologist and approached the process of cognitive development from this perspective. He explained cognitive development by applying the ideas that biologists use to explain physical development.

Piaget’s theory was created through a series of experiments, which allowed him to conclude that cognitive development occurs through the “interaction of inborn abilities and environmental events that proceed through a series of stages of intellectual development”.

Piaget therefore saw intelligence as a process, with individuals learning about the world around them and how to interact with it, and intelligence being a state of balance achieved by an individual when they are able to deal adequately with the environment around them.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Explain how Piaget developed his theory.

A

Jean Piaget was a renowned psychologist and cognitive theorist in the 20th century who focused on child development. His theories came from observing children and recording their development.
He brought attention to the idea that children are not just small adults, and he argued that the way they think is fundamentally different.
Piaget believed that children act as “little scientists,” exploring their environment to gain understanding. He thought that children do this naturally, without any adult intervention. He put forth the idea of distinct developmental stages through which children learn language, memory, and reasoning.
This article explains Piaget’s four stages of cognitive development, key concepts, and how people can use them to help children learn and develop.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Explain Piaget’s concepts of schemas, assimilation, accommodation, adaptation and disequilibrium/equilibrium.

A

Piaget’s concepts of schemas, assimilation, accommodation and disequilibrium/equilibrium.

(1). Schemas (In relation to Piaget’s theory):

You met the idea ofschemain your study of memory in Unit 2. In that context, it was used to explain why eyewitnesses’ memories are not always accurate. We use our schemas to aid us in remembering: we have expectations and we use them to interpret and process what we see. Schemas play a similar role in cognitive development: if our schemas are not adequate they are changed. The starting point is the schemas (often called reflex schemas) which are present at birth.

(2). Assimilation, accommodation and adaptation:

Three concepts are crucial in understanding Piaget’s theory as to how mental processes develop:

Assimilation - refers to fitting new experiences into existing schemas.

Accommodation -refers to altering existing schemas to fit in with new experiences.

Adaptation -refers to accommodation and assimilation working together.

Functional invariants & variant structures:

Functional invariants: - structures that stay the same throughout the development process, which assist in the discovery and understanding of knowledge. There are two of these:

1.The process of adaptation – involves accommodation and assimilation.

  1. The process of equilibration – involves swinging between equilibrium and disequilibrium.

Variant structures: - structures that change and develop as knowledge is discovered. There are two of these:

1.Schemas – units of intelligence/ ways of understanding the world.

  1. Operations – strings of schemas assembled in a logical order.

(3). Disequilibrium/equilibrium:

When a person’s mental processes cannot cope with new experiences they are said to be indisequilibrium; when accommodation has occurred,equilibriumis restored.

The process of swinging between equilibrium and disequilibrium is calledequilibration.

In psychology and in the theory of disequilibrium and accommodation,equalibriumrefers to a person’s background knowledge (schema) that allows the individual to understand the new information through assimilation.Assimilationis a process of applying and understanding new information with existing knowledge without needing to change the existing knowledge. In this sense, the new information and existing information are considered to be in an equilibrium state.
For example, a child may learn that an apple is round, has red skin and white flesh, and has a sweet scent. When he or she sees a fruit on the dining table that suits all the criteria, he or she will understand it is an apple with his or her existing knowledge. This process is called assimilation, or putting new information in a new situation into the existing schema.

Disequilibrium Psychology

In psychology,disequalibriumrefers to when the new information cannot fit into the existing schema. When this situation happens, equilibrium occurs. When this happens, one can decide whether a new concept should be built (accommodation), or whether the existing schema can be modified to allow the new information to fit in.

In context:

Firstly however, it is important to identify a babies earliest schemas which are produced before the later stages of cognitive development.
A baby’s earliest schemas are inborn reflexes, such as sucking.

These give babies something to interact with the environments and thus help discover knowledge with. Early schemas are external and physical, while later schemas are more internal and cognitive (mental). These are the earliest forms of thinking, over time they become less reflex and more deliberate and under the infant’s control.

} Cognitive development can be seen through the example of the innate schema of sucking. – At first babies suck everything they come into contact with in the same inborn manner. This is called assimilation (part of the process of adaption, as we have seen) and involves fitting new environmental experiences into existing schemas. If this is possible, infants are in a state of equilibrium (part of the process of equilibrium), a pleasant state of balance.

} When infants find something new so that they cannot suck in the usual way, such as drinking from a cup, they experience disequilibrium (the other part of the process of equilibrium), an unpleasant state of imbalance. Children are naturally motivated to return to the balanced state of equilibrium, achieved by accommodation of the new experience (the other part of the process of adaption).

This involves altering existing schemas to accommodate (fit in) new experiences, such as using new lip shapes to drink/ suck out of different things.

} Therefore, cognitive development involves constantly swinging between equilibrium and disequilibrium, through continuous series of assimilation (fitting knowledge into new schemas) and accommodation (altering schemas to fit in new knowledge). When new schemas are formed, assimilation allows for the practice of the new experiences until they are automatic. This process continues through life, but is most apparent on the first 15 years.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Describe Piaget’s stages of cognitive development.

A

Piaget’s stages of cognitive development.

Piaget recognised that children appear to go through stages in their cognitive development. He proposed that these stages are universal across cultures and that all children go through the stages in the same order.

Piaget proposed four major stages:

Sensorimotorstage(birth to 2 years)

Pre-operational stage (2–7 years)

Concrete operational stage (7–11 years)

Formal operational stage (11+) years

Piaget described the characteristics that are possessed (or not possessed) by children at each of these stages.

  1. The Sensorimotor stage (the first stage: birth to 2 years).

At the start of this stage, babies only ‘think’ when perceiving or acting on objects. They have no internal representation of objects or events that adults would recognise as thinking. Therefore, when an object or events that adults would recognise as thinking. Therefore, when an object is not being perceived or acted on, it does not exist – that is, the baby has no object permanence. Infants at this point are highly egocentric, as they have no understanding of the world other than their own, but gradually during this stage they come to realise what objects exist and events occur independent of their behaviour; they can then be said to have acquired object permanence.

Initially between one and four months of age, babies will look at the place where objects disappear from for a few moments, but will not search of them. Then babies start to reach for partially hidden objects – suggesting that they realise that the rest of the object is attached to the visible part. At the age of one, babies search for hidden objects, but only where they were last seen. Object permanence is not fully formed until two years of age.

The other development in the sensorimotor stage is the is the establishment of the general symbolic function (GSF). – This occurs in the final part of the stage, involving the development of mental images to represent objects that children have experienced. Children are no longer dependant of physical manipulation of objects to ‘think’ about them; they can ‘think” inside their heads, making a transition of schemas to being internal rather than external.

As you will see, thesensorimotorstage can be subdivided into six sub-stages, each based on a new skill as shown in Table 1.1.
Table 1.1Sensorimotorsub-stages:

Sub-stage: Skill: Age (months):
1. Reflexes. 0–1
2. Primary circular reactions. 1–4
3. Secondary circular reactions. 4–8
4. Coordination of reactions. 8–12
5. Tertiary circular reactions. 12–18
6. Early representational thought. 18–24

  1. Pre-operational stage.

During the pre-operational stage the GSF continues to develop.

The term ‘pre-operational’ is used here because of the child’s reliance on how an object ‘seems’ rather than using logical principles (mental operations). This leads to some significant problems with reasoning. For example, a child might point to an apple and say ‘apple’ but also say ‘apple’ when pointing to an orange: he or she has associated apples with being a ‘round fruit’ and so oranges must also be apples. If Mummy goes into hospital to have a baby and then Daddy goes into a hospital then he also is going to have a baby.

This lack of reasoning is characterised by:

centration

transductive reasoning

animistic thinking

seriation.

Notice that the pre-operational stage can be divided into pre-conceptual and intuitive sub-stages:
Pre-operational stage (2 to 7 years):

} Centraction – children cannot classify things in a logical manner. (See above as well).

} Transductive reasoning – relationships between two objects are based o a single attribute, for example a dog has four legs, a cat has four legs, therefore a cat is a dog and vide versa.

} Animalistic thinking – the belief that inanimate objects are alive.
} Seriation – children find it hard to put items in order, for example of size; theycan perceive only biggest or smallest.

Conservation:

During this stage children have problems with conservation, which is the ability to realise that the physical properties of an object, such as volume, stay the same despite the object changing its appearance.

Centration could explain the lack of conservation ability because a child cannot attend to two physical attributes at the same time. For example, if you pour liquid from one vessel into another and the vessels differ in width, you will see the liquid change in appearance. The level of the liquid will rise if the second vessel has a narrower width (see Figure 5.7 in your textbook).

As an adult you know that the height has increased but the width has decreased so the volume stays the same. A pre-operational child can only focus on one attribute and so thinks because the liquid in the second vessel has an increased height it must have a larger volume.

Egocentrism:

Children in the pre-operational stage are highly egocentric, viewing the world from their own viewpoint; this results in inabilities in their reasoning.
The ‘three mountains‘ task classically allowed children to demonstrate their level ofegocentrism.

Brief description: Children were shown a 3D model of three mountains, each with a different object on top. They were asked what the view would be like for a ‘doll’ placed at different positions around the model. The child is demonstratingegocentrismif they describe, or choose a picture, showing their own viewpoint rather than the doll’s.

  1. Concrete operational stage

During the concrete operational stage children start to develop mental structures called ‘operations’. However, children can only perform these mental operations if they have objects physically present, which is why the stage is called ‘concrete’.

Conservation requires the ability to carry out mental operations and during this stage children develop this ability as long as they have access to physical objects. The ability to conserve the different physical characteristics is acquired in the same order for every child. This step-by-step acquisition is calleddecalage. There can be inconsistenciesbetweenabilities as well aswithinabilities; for instance, a child may master classification but not all of conservation.

Class inclusion:

Class inclusionis about being able to see that objects can belong to different categories at the same time. For example, if you had 12 flowers made up of four daffodils and eight roses and you were asked if there were more flowers or more roses, you would answer more flowers. A child who can’t accomplish class inclusion would say more roses. This is not due to the fact that they don’t know what a rose or a daffodil is or that they don’t know what constitutes a flower. Nor is it due to an inability to count.

As with conservation tasks, the ability to decentre is required before a person can be successful in class inclusion tasks. One has to consider two aspects of the objects at the same time to work out that ALL the flowers include the roses AND the daffodils but the roses are just one group of flowers.

The ability to carry out class inclusion tasks develops as children become able to classify objects as belonging to two or more categories at the same time.
Children lose theiregocentrismas they progress through the concrete operational stage. This is calleddecentring.

Some types of conservation are mastered earlier than others and the other in which this occurs is invariant (the same for everybody):

· Liquid quantity develops by age 6-7.

· Substance/quantity and length by 7-8 years.

· Weight by 8-7 years.

· Volume by 11-12 years.

  1. Formal operational stage:

In the formal operation stage children can manipulate abstract ideas and so can now manage hypothetical situations. They can accomplish mental arithmetic and tackle inferential reasoning.

The age at which people complete this final stage varies enormously and it is said that some never complete it at all.

Formal operational stage (11+ years):

Manipulating objects concerns first operations, but children in the formal operations stage can now perform second-order operations where they manipulate ideas.

Hypothetical situations can be thought about – that is, possibilities, not actualities. Children develop the ability to manipulate things in their head without any need for an object to be physically viewable, for example mathematical sums can be performed mentally without any need for physical manipulation of objects.

  • Inferential reasoning can now also take place where conclusions can be drawn about things that haven’t actually been experienced. For example, if a child knows that person A is taller than person B and that person B is taller than person C, it can be concluded that person A is taller than person C even though persons A and C have never been experienced together.

Abstract reasoning of concepts and ideas with no physical presence also becomes apparent and children can consider possible outcomes of situations rather than having to rely on previous experience. This is important for being able to form long-term plans and goals.

This stage is not as uniform as others. Some reach it by the age of 15, others by age 20, some maybe not at all, and different individuals develop formal operations in different areas of ability and experience. This stage therefore seems not to be as genetically controlled as others. Some researchers believe as few as one third of people reach this stage.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

PC: T1. Piaget’s theory of Cognitive development: Outline the characteristics of object permanence, conservation,egocentrismand class inclusion.

A

The chrarasterstics of object permanence, conservation, egocentrism and class inclusion.

  1. Object permanence:

The main development during thesensorimotor stageis the understanding that objects exist and events occur in the world independently of one’s own actions (‘the object concept’, or ‘object permanence’).

Object permanence means knowing that an object still exists, even if it is hidden. It requires the ability to form a mental representation (i.e. a schema) of the object.

For example, if you place a toy under a blanket, the child who has achieved object permanence knows it is there and can actively seek it. At the beginning of this stage the child behaves as if the toy had simply disappeared.
The attainment of object permanence generally signals the transition from the sensorimotor stage to thepreoperational stage of development.

Blanket and Ball Study:

Aim: Piaget (1963) wanted to investigate at what age children acquire object permanence.

Method: Piaget hid a toy under a blanket, while the child was watching, and observed whether or not the child searched for the hidden toy.
Searching for the hidden toy was evidence of object permanence. Piaget assumed that the child could only search for a hidden toy if s/he had a mental representation of it.

Results: Piaget found that infants searched for the hidden toy when they were around 8-months-old.

Conclusion: Children around 8 months have object permanence because they are able to form a mental representation of the object in their minds.

Evaluation: Piaget assumed the results of his study occur because the children under 8 months did not understand that the object still existed underneath the blanket (and therefore did not reach for it). However, there are alternative reasons why a child may not search for an object rather than lack of understanding of situation.

  • The child could become distracted or lose interest in the object and therefore lack the motivation to search for it, or simply may not have the physical coordination to carry out the motor movements necessary for the retrieval of the object (Mehler & Dupoux, 1994).

The A-not-B Error:

The A-not-B error occurs when infants search for a hidden toy at the incorrect location when presented with two possible locations (Piaget, 1954).

The toy is repeatedly hidden at location A. After a short delay, infants are then allowed to reach for and retrieve the toy.

After a few trials the toy is then clearly hidden in location B. After a short delay, they are then allowed to reach for the toy.

Infants 8 to 10 months of age consistently reach to location A despite clearly seeing the toy hidden at location B.

Critical Evaluation:

There is evidence that object permanence occurs earlier than Piaget claimed. Bower and Wishart (1972) used a lab experiment to study infants aged between 1 – 4 months old.

Instead of using a Piaget’s blanket technique they waited for the infant to reach for an object, and then turned out the lights so that the object was no longer visible. They then filmed the infant using an infrared camera. They found that the infant continued to reach for the object for up to 90 seconds after it became invisible.

Again, just like Piaget’s study there are also criticisms of Bower’s ‘reaching in the dark’ findings. Each child had up to 3 minutes to complete the task and reach for the object. Within this time period, it is plausible they may have successfully completed the task by accident.

For example, randomly reaching out and finding the object or even reaching out due to the distress of the lights going out (rather than reaching out with the intention of searching for an object).

  1. Conservation:

During this stage children have problems with conservation, which is the ability to realise that the physical properties of an object, such as volume, stay the same despite the object changing its appearance.

Centration could explain the lack of conservation ability because a child cannot attend to two physical attributes at the same time. For example, if you pour liquid from one vessel into another and the vessels differ in width, you will see the liquid change in appearance. The level of the liquid will rise if the second vessel has a narrower width (see Figure 5.7 in your textbook).

As an adult you know that the height has increased but the width has decreased so the volume stays the same. A pre-operational child can only focus on one attribute and so thinks because the liquid in the second vessel has an increased height it must have a larger volume.

  1. Egocentrism:

Children in the pre-operational stage are highly egocentric, viewing the world from their own viewpoint; this results in inabilities in their reasoning.
The ‘three mountains‘ task classically allowed children to demonstrate their level ofegocentrism.

Brief description: Children were shown a 3D model of three mountains, each with a different object on top. They were asked what the view would be like for a ‘doll’ placed at different positions around the model. The child is demonstratingegocentrismif they describe, or choose a picture, showing their own viewpoint rather than the doll’s.

More recent studies have attempted to ask questions more clearly and to present situations to which children can relate more easily.

Evaluation:Policeman Doll Study:

Martin Hughes (1975) argued that the three mountains task did not make sense to children and was made more difficult because the children had to match the doll’s view with a photograph.

Hughes devised a task which made sense to the child. He showed children a model comprising two intersecting walls, a ‘boy’ doll and a ‘policeman’ doll. He then placed the policeman doll in various positions and asked the child to hide the boy doll from the policeman.

Hughes did this to make sure that the child understood what was being asked of him, so if s/he made mistakes they were explained and the child tried again. Interestingly, very few mistakes were made.

The experiment then began. Hughes brought in a second policeman doll, and placed both dolls at the end of two walls, as shown in the illustration above.
The child was asked to hide the boy from both policemen, in other words he had to take account of two different points of view.

Hughes’ sample comprised children between three and a half and five years of age, of whom 90 percent gave correct answers. Even when he devised a more complex situation, with more walls and a third policeman, 90 percent of four-year-olds were successful.

This shows that children have largely lost their egocentric thinking by four years of age, because they are able to take the view of another.
Hughes’ experiment allowed them to demonstrate this because the task made sense to the child, whereas Piaget’s did not. Suggesting that differences in ‘meaning’ children ascribe to situation might cause them to pass or fail task.

Egocentrism, inpsychology, thecognitiveshortcomings that underlie the failure, in bothchildrenand adults, to recognize theidiosyncraticnature of one’s knowledge or the subjective nature of one’s perceptions. Such failures describe children at play who cover their eyes and joyfully exclaim to their parents, “You can’t see me!” Likewise, they describe adult physicians who provide their patients with medicaldiagnosesthat only another doctor could understand.

  1. Class inclusion:

Class inclusionis about being able to see that objects can belong to different categories at the same time. For example, if you had 12 flowers made up of four daffodils and eight roses and you were asked if there were more flowers or more roses, you would answer more flowers. A child who can’t accomplish class inclusion would say more roses. This is not due to the fact that they don’t know what a rose or a daffodil is or that they don’t know what constitutes a flower. Nor is it due to an inability to count.

As with conservation tasks, the ability to decentre is required before a person can be successful in class inclusion tasks. One has to consider two aspects of the objects at the same time to work out that ALL the flowers include the roses AND the daffodils but the roses are just one group of flowers.

The ability to carry out class inclusion tasks develops as children become able to classify objects as belonging to two or more categories at the same time.
Children lose theiregocentrismas they progress through the concrete operational stage. This is calleddecentring.

Concrete operational stage (7 to 11 years):

Conservation is an example of an operation, with concrete operational children able to understand the relationship between the whole of something and its parts.

Some types of conservation are mastered earlier than others and the other in which this occurs is invariant (the same for everybody):

· Liquid quantity develops by age 6-7.

· Substance/quantity and length by 7-8 years.

· Weight by 8-7 years.

· Volume by 11-12 years.

This step-by-step acquisition of new operations is called decalage (as stated above).

With conservation, decalage is horizontal, as there are inconsistencies within the same kind of ability or operation – for example, a seven year old can conserve number but not weight. There is also vertical decalage, which refers to inconsistencies between different abilities or operations – for example, children may master all kinds of classification but not all kinds of conservation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

PC: T2. Baillargeon and early infant abilities: Explain Baillargeon’s theory about early infant knowledge of the physical world.

A

Baillargeon’s theory about early infant knowledge of the physical world.

Renée Baillargeon was born in 1954 and so had the advantage of not only knowing about Piaget’s theories but also being familiar with the research carried out by others in evaluating Piaget’s ideas.

She was interested in the age at which children acquired object permanence. She devised the ‘violation of expectation’ technique that tests at what age children develop object permanence.

Baillargeon used inventive methods as it is very difficult to assess what young infants know and do not know, due to their inability to coherently explain their thoughts. In addition, infants cannot always demonstrate their knowledge and understanding in their behavior, because their motor skills are not developed sufficiently.

Baillargeon agrees with Piaget’s theory in that she suggests that a child’s understanding occurs through the primitive innate knowledge being challenged through experience of the world. However, as with Piaget she might be misinterpreting a child’s behaviour.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

PC: T2. Baillargeon and early infant abilities: Explain the role of Core Knowledge Theory (CKT).

A

The role of Core Knowledge Theory (CKT).

The experiments you have just read about demonstrate that children do have an ability to appreciate object permanence. They also show that children are born with a number of systems ofcore knowledgewhich allow appreciation of the relationships between inanimate objects.

This basic understanding of the physical world includes such properties as the following:

Solid objects cannot pass through each other; they each occupy their own space.

Objects move from A to B in space and they have continuity of motion.

These rules are rather primitive at birth and become more sophisticated as the child develops.

Baillargeon’s core explanation for core knowledge was that: humans have an innate understanding of inanimate objects and their relationships with each other.

Baillargeon’s research has helped to investigate a major aspect of the core knowledge theory, that of object representation.

The CKT believes that humans are born with a small number of systems of core knowledge that serve to represent inanimate objects and their relationships with each other.

– The idea of object representation is that infants have an inborn ability to perceive the boundaries of objects, to be able to still perceive objects when they move partially or completely out of view and to be able to predict the movements of objects.

  • So just like animals such as horses have an innate ability to stand and walk, humans are seen to have an innate ability to understand objects. Thus humans are seen to possess and understanding of object permanence at a far younger age than Piaget believed, possibly even from birth, and this understanding allows them to quickly learn from environmental experiences, which has an evolutionary survival value

Recent research, however, has cast doubt on the conclusions drawn by Baillargeon, thus reducing support for CKT and the idea that children from a very early age posses a sense of object permanence.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

PC: T2. Baillargeon and early infant abilities: Explain the use of Baillargeon’s violation of expectation in research to ascertain early infant abilities.

A

The use of Baillargeon’s violation of expectation (VOE) in research to ascertain early infant abilities.

Violation of expectation technique (VOE).

Theviolation of expectation technique (VOE)relies on the fact that children will look for a longer time at novel objects than at familiar objects. The technique involves a child being repeatedly shown an unfamiliar scenario until they start to look away, demonstrating that it is no longer a novel experience. Like other research, this shows that children demonstrate object permanence at a much earlier age than was suggested by Piaget.

Baillargeon and her co-workers devised several ‘magic’ tricks to discover whether children showed surprise when they saw the impossible happen. If children acted with surprise they assumed that this was because they knew the difference between what was possible and impossible.

  • Research showed that infants would continue to look at the unlikely occurrence for longer and infants as young as 3 and a half months of age would be able to begin to comprehend the object permanence and not look at the event for such a long time.

It is easier to understand these experiments by watching them in action. YouTube has several videos, including:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=l1VK2iawS34(3:30; Minnie Mouse doll experiments)

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hwgo2O5Vk_g(2:34; ramp set-up)

Several different set-ups were used:

A drawbridge that moved through 180 degrees. A box was used as an obstacle to the drawbridge. The ‘magic’ situation was to show the drawbridge still able to move despite the box being in the way.

A truck rolling down a ramp; again, a box is used as an obstacle.

A tall and a short carrot passing behind a screen with a window in it. Figure 15.12 in your textbook illustrates this set-up.

A Minnie Mouse passing behind a screen and re-emerging on the other side. Figure 15.13 illustrates the variations on the Minnie Mouse set-up.

Each of these experiments relies on the same premise, i.e. that one trial demonstrates an impossible set-up. For each one the observer is looking for any sign that the child appreciates that an impossible event is occurring.

Baillargeon’s explanation for early infant abilities – a theory that sees children as able to understand the properties of objects and their relationships to each other from an early age.

Baillargeon was inspired by research that appeared to suggest children develop object permanence at a much earlier age than Piaget believed, such as Bower & Wisheart’s (1972) study, as explained above, that find nine-month-old babies show surprise when toys disappear, Baillargeon was able to assess Piaget’s beliefs about the ages at which children reach various levels of cognitive development by creation of her violation of expectation technique (VOE).

  • Baillargeon, either solely or in collaboration with others, has gone on to perform a series of experiments based upon her violation of expectation technique (VOE) that uses scenarios familiar to or understandable by very young infants to consistently find results that challenge Piaget’s ideas and suggest that children much younger than he thought demonstrate object permanence – and have an intuitive knowledge concerning the properties of objects and their relationships to each other.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

PC: T2. Baillargeon and early infant abilities: Evaluation of Baillargeon’s theory.

A

Evaluation of Baillargeon’s theory.

Cashon and Cohen (2000) – familiarised eight-month-old infants with one of four Baillargeon-type scenarios, which involved a screen rotating in either a 180 or 130 degree arc and a block that was either placed in the path of the rotating screen or was absent from the scenario. It was found that infants looked for longer at scenarios that were more interesting (had more visual novelty) than ones that were ‘impossible’ which reduces support for the idea that young infants have object permanence and that humans have an innate understanding of object representation as proposed by the CKT.

Research evaluation:

Schöner and Thelen (2004) – also believe that Baillargeon drew conclusions that went beyond thedata.
For instance, they point out that in the drawbridge study it could be the extra movement that exists in the ‘impossible’ situation that attracts children. So it might be confoundingvariablesthat are influencing the outcome rather than a true measure of a child’s ability in terms of object permanence and knowledge of the physical world.

*Nonetheless, Baillargeon’s technique is viewed as aparadigm method;this means that it is an accepted method of assessing children’s understanding of the physical world. Baillargeon’s methods have achieved consistent results and the CKT is now widely accepted as a valid explanation.

(Note: when evaluating Baillargeon’s work, remember to mention the paradigm method in conclusion to your evaluation, as this is the crux consequence of her research.

Additional evaluative points:

· Investigating the cognitive developmental abilities of infants is problematic as such children cannot communicate verbally what abilities they have. Therefore Baillargeon’s paradigm technique is extremely useful, as it allows insight into what skills very young children possess.

· Research based on their Baillargeon’s methodology produces consistent results and the CKT (central knowledge theory) has come to be widely accepted as a valid explanation.
– However, some believe Baillargeon draws conclusions that go beyond what the data shows, as stated above with Schöner and Thelen (2004). And therefore (from the research stared above) what Baillargeon sees as evidence of infants having innate knowledge of object representation may actually be just the effect of confounding variables, this research is supported by Cashon and Cohen (2000).

· Aguair and Baillargeon’s (1999) (see example in online textbook) Minnie Mouse study is a clever method of investigating the inability of children under three months of age to show surprise at the failure of the tall carrot to appear in the window in the upper half of the screen in Baillargeon and DeVos’s (1991) study.
– Baillargeon wondered whether children of that age did not have a sense of object permanence or instead had not learned that when an object moves behind something that obscures its bottom half, the height of the object determines whether it will be visible in the top half of the window. In the Minnie Mouse study only her bottom half should be visible, something that two-and-a-half-month-old children understood, suggesting that they did not have a sense of object permanence.

Subsequent points:

The VOE technique relies on the premise that children will look for longer at novel situations than familiar ones.

Minnie Mouse passes behind a screen with windows in it and it is set up so that she appears to disappear when she shouldn’t. Children looked longer at this impossible set-up, demonstrating that they knew she did exist and should have been visible. Children showed this response at an earlier age than Piaget had suggested that object permanence develops.

Core knowledge theory is that humans are born with a small number of systems of core knowledge that serve to represent inanimate objects. In relation to object permanence, infants have an inborn ability to perceive boundaries of objects, so that they can continue to perceive that object even if it is partially or totally out of view.

A paradigm is when there is universal acceptance of a particular way of thinking. Baillargeon’s VOE technique has been taken on board in psychological research as an acceptable way of assessing children’s understanding of the physical world.

Cashon and Cohen suggested that children were looking at situations longer due to their interest rather than to their novelty. This challenged the idea that children possessed object permanence at this early age.

Both Piaget and Baillargeon’s theories support the idea that a child’s understanding occurs through primitive innate knowledge being challenged through experience of the world.

Summary of Baillargeon and early infant abilities.

In this topic you have seen how Baillargeon devised the technique of violation of expectation. Using this, children were shown to have object permanence abilities at an earlier age than suggested by Piaget’s theory. Baillargeon used the core knowledge theory to explain how it is possible for humans to possess object permanence at birth.

A criticism of Baillargeon’s technique was that children are keener to watch some situations due to their interest rather than simply their novelty. This means that children are not necessarily showing object permanence in her VOE technique.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

PC: T3. Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development: Describe/ outline Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development.

A

Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development.

Piaget was criticised for ignoring the social situation within which children’s cognitions develop. Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934), on the other hand, saw that the influence of culture is crucial to cognitive development and believed that children benefited from aid being given to scaffold their learning.

Vygotsky and cognitive development:

In 1990, Freund carried out an investigation where children had to decide which items of furniture belonged in a certain area of a doll’s house. Some children had an earlier experience of working on a similar task with theirmotherwhereas other children worked alone with no prior experience. Freund discovered that the children who had prior experience of performing a joint task with their mothers did better than the children who carried out the task totally alone.

Vygotsky and Piaget’s theories are in agreement that knowledge is constructed from a child’s active interaction with their environment, but Vygotsky mainly saw cognitive development as a cultural construct – that is, affected by the learning of norms and attitudes of whichever culture a child is raised within.

  • For Vygotsky, therefore, cognitive development is a social process and he aimed to explain how higher mental functions – reasoning, understanding, etc. – arise out of children’s social experiences by considering development mainly in terms of cultural and interpersonal levels.

The first thing that might have struck you is how long ago Vygotsky’s theory was first developed. Vygotsky lived in the first few decades of the twentieth century but his work wasn’t widely known until the 1970s. When you look into his theory you will appreciate its continuing relevance despite its being nearly 100 years old. Vygotsky died young and so did not have time to fully develop his own theory, unlike the long working life experienced by Piaget.

Vygotsky’s work has therefore been thoroughly researched and developed by other psychologists, especially Jerome Bruner who is largely responsible for the concept of scaffolding, which you will meet later in this topic.

Vygotsky’s theory agrees with Piaget’s that knowledge is constructed through interactions with the environment. However, it differs in that it proposes that cognitive development is a social process.

  • Vygotsky saw the interactions of children with their caregivers as a way for them to acquire cultural attitudes and beliefs, inheriting a number of cultural tools.

The cultural level:
Children are seen as benefiting from the knowledge of previous generations, which they gain through interactions with caregivers, thus passing cultural attitudes and beliefs from one generation to another, developing them further, then handling them on to the next generation.
For Vygotsky, the most essential cultural tool is language.

The Interpersonal level:

At the interpersonal level, culture and the individuals meet. Cognitive development occurs first on a social level, through interaction between people (interpsycholgical). Even higher-level cognitive functions and concepts were seen by Vygotsky as originating through interactions between individuals.

The basis of Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development – is that an explanation of the growth of intellect that sees intellectual growth as a social process where cognitive functions arise from children’s interactions with others.

Summary of internalisation and social nature of thinking:

The ability to think and reason by and for oneself is called inner speech or verbal thought. Infants are born as social beings capable of interacting with others, but able to do little practically or intellectually by themselves. Gradually, children become more self-sufficient and independent, and by participating in social activities their abilities develop.

For Vygotsky, cognitive development involves active internalisation of problem-solving processes, taking place as a result of mutual interaction between children and people they have social contact with. This therefore replaces Piaget’s ‘child as a scientist’ idea (that children construct and test out their own theories about the world) with the ‘child as an apprentice’ idea, where cultural skills and knowledge are gained through collaboration with those who possess them.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

PC: T3. Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development: Outline Vygotsky: similarities/ differences to Piaget’s development beliefs .

A

Difference:

Unlike Piaget’s notion that childrens’ development must necessarily precede their learning, Vygotsky argued, “learning is a necessary and universal aspect of the process of developing culturally organized, specifically human psychological function” - In other words, social learning tends to precede (i.e., come before) development.

Similarity:

Like Piaget, Vygotsky could be described as aconstructivist, in that he was interested in knowledge acquisition as a cumulative event - with new experiences and understandings incorporated into existing cognitive frameworks. However, whilst Piaget’s theory is structural (arguing that development is governed by physiological stages), Vygotsky denies the existence of any guiding framework independent of culture and context.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

PC: T3. Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development: Explain what is meant by thezone of proximal development.

A

Vygotsky was best known for his sociocultural theory. He constructed the idea of azone of proximal development,which are those tasks which are too difficult for a child to solve alone but s/he can accomplish with the help of adults or more skilled peers.

The zone of proximal development:

This sensitivity to the child’s abilities relies on recognising thezone of proximal development(ZPD)described by Vygotsky. - This refers to recognising where the child is in its accomplishments, i.e. what he or she can do with no help, what requires a little help and what is beyond the realm of possibility and so needs to be carried out by someone else.

Vygotsky’s theory talks about ‘experts’ helping the child, but this does not need to be a person who knows a lot more than the child; in fact, the closer that person is to the child in terms of knowledge the more likely they are to offer the appropriate assistance.

This means that the ‘teacher’ can be a peer, if they have progressed that bit more than the ‘learner’. If the ‘teacher’ is an adult whose knowledge is far superior, then they must be able to empathise with the learner and put themselves in the learner’s shoes to appreciate their present position.

The mantra for Vygotsky’s theory is ‘what a child can do with help today it can do on its own tomorrow’. If you do everything for a child it will never learn to do things for itself; if you give a child tasks beyond its scope then frustration will ensue. The intelligent aspect of this is to pitch activities for children just right to fall within the child’s ZPD.

SLT states that learning is accomplished throughmimickingsomeone else’s behaviour, therefore you would not instruct at all but just allow the child to watch as you tie your laces. You might slow the process down and ensure that the child is paying attention to what you are doing, but you would not allow the child any part in the task. After a number of demonstrations you would then expect the child to be able to perform the whole task in one go.

The problem with this approach is that it is not efficient in learning a complex task involving several sub-steps. To allow a child to achieve each sub-step in an incremental way is both more efficient and reduces frustration which might lead the child to give up on the task.

The Zone of proximal development, in short is the distance between current and potential ability. Cultural influences and knowledgeable others push children through the ZPD and on to tasks beyond their current ability.

Research support:

  • Wood and Middleton (1975) – observed mothers using various strategies to support four year olds in building a model that was too difficult for the children to do themselves. Mothers who were most effective in offering assistance were ones who varied their strategy according to how well a child was doing, so that when a child was progressing well they gave less specific help, until the child made progress again. – this highlights the concept of the ZPD and shows that scaffolding (involves children being assisted by mentors who give clues as to how to solve problems rather than giving actual solutions) also is most effective when matched to the needs of a learner, so that they are assisted to achieve success in a task that previously they could not have completed alone.

The use of ZPD shows that it is most effective when matched with other learning techniques such as scaffolding, these can then also be tailored to the needs of the learner.

· Studies have shown that full demonstrations are less effective for children, because the adults are imposing a solution on to the children with too difficult verbal.

Instructions and too many details. From this it was then seen that children learned better when given more independence, and where the mothers only provided assistance when the children got stuck. This made it easier for the children to not find the situation so imposing, and still have guidance when they need it.

This use of ZPD supports Vygotsky’s theory – that knowledgeable tutors can help to assist learners on to a higher level of learning.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

PC: T3. Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development: Explain the role of scaffolding in developing a child’s cognitive ability and the role of language.

A

The role of scaffolding in developing a child’s cognitive ability.

  1. Scaffolding:

Vygotsky’s Scaffolding concept isa theory that teachers can use as a tool for growth. It focuses on the learner’s ability to grow through the guidance of a more informed individual. Working in collaboration with a guide or with a peer group can help students achieve independent learning.

Scaffolding sees cognitive development as being assisted by sensitive guidance, with children being given more clues as to how to solve a problem, rather then being given an actual solution.

With scaffolding, learning first involves shared social activities until individuals can self-scaffold, with learning eventually becoming an individual, self-regulated activities.

Several processes have been identified that help create effective scaffolding:
1. Ensuring a task is easy.
2. Gaining and maintaining a child’s interest in the task.
3. Demonstrating the task.
4. Keeping a child’s level of frustration under control.
5. Stressing elements that will help create a solution to a task.

In context:

In the teaching of tying shoelaces, scaffolding is achieved by breaking the task down into steps and then intervening at each step in a sequential manner.

This gives the child support when it needs it and takes it away when it is no longer needed. Without the support the child would fail; if the support was never removed the child would not be forced to try it alone and so learn the task.

Hopefully you have not had too many experiences when a teacher did not provide suitable scaffolding, but you should now be able to see the relevance of Vygotsky’s theory. For example, scaffolding is a much safer approach to teaching someone to drive than the SLT approach.

Research:
- McNaughton & Leyland (1990) – observed mothers giving increasingly explicit help to children assembling progressively harder jigsaws, which illustrates how scaffolding and sensitivity to a child’s ZPD aid learning.

  1. The role of language:

Both Piaget and Vygotsky commented on how language is related to cognitive development and, as you might expect, the two theories differ.

Piaget felt that cognitions precede language and so he did not put emphasis on advancing cognitive development by encouraging language. Vygotsky, on the other hand, saw language as an important element in allowing cognitive development to progress.
Vygotsky identified pre-intellectual language and pre-linguistic thought occurring before the age of 2 years; the two then merge, with speech and thinking becoming interdependent and thinking being internal conversation.

Stages and concept formation:

As with Piaget, Vygotsky recognised stages in cognitive development. He described stages of concept formation quite similar to Piaget’s stages:

  1. vague syncretic.
  2. complex.
  3. potential concept.
    4.mature concept.

Vygotsky (1934) gave children blocks with nonsense symbols on them and they had to work out what the symbols meant. Four different approaches were observed, from which Vygotsky proposed four stages concept formation.

Detail of the four stages of concept formation from the textbook table:

Vague Syncretic = Trial-and-error formation of concepts without comprehension of them. Similar to Piaget’s pre-operational stage.

Complex = Use of some strategies to comprehend concepts, but not very systematic.

Potential concept = More systematic use of strategies, with one attribute being focused on acts time – for example: weight.

Mature concept = Several attributes can be dealt with systematically – for example: weight and colour. Similar to Piaget’s formal operations.

Semiotics – The use of signs and symbols:

Semiotics involves the use of signs and symbols to create meaning. Vygotsky saw semiotics as assisting cognitive development through the use of language and other cultural symbols, which act as a medium for knowledge to be transmitted, turning elementary mental functions into higher ones.

  • At first, children use pre-intellectual language for social and emotional purposes, where words are not symbols for they represent but instead reflect properties of the objects, with pre-linguistic thinking occurring without the use of language.
  • From around two years of age, language and thinking combine so that speech and thought become independent (dependant on each other), with thinking thus becoming an internal conversation.

Such development occurs in severe phases:
1. Social speech (birth to 3 years) – involving pre-intellectual language.
2. Egocentric speech (birth 3-7 years) involving self-talk/thinking aloud.
3. Inner speech (7+ years) – Where self-talk becomes silent and internal, and language is used for social communications.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

PC: T3. Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development: Ao3 - evaluate Vygotsky’s theory and provide evidence both for and against.

A

Evaluation of Vygotsky’s theory.

The evaluation of Vygotsky can be separated into several areas, such as culture issues:

  1. Culture-fairness: sensitive guidance, scaffolding and ZPD are applicable in all cultures.
  2. It can explain the influence of culture on cognitive development.
  3. It was developed within a collectivist culture so maybe it is more suited to cultures which emphasise social interactions, rather than to cultures which are more individualistic.

It might be possible to combine Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories to give a fuller explanation of cognitive development. Both theories describe similar stages and have stimulated much research. They both have important applications to education.

Summary evaluation (main points from textbook: further information):

· Different cultures empathise different skills and learning goals and yet Vygotsky’s concepts of sensitive guidance, scaffolding and ZPD applicable in all cultures, suggesting his concepts to be ‘culture-fair’.

· There are strong central similarities between Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories and is has been suggested that combining the two, as said above, may be feasible and desirable to gain a fuller understanding of children’s cognitive development.

· Vygotsky’s theory is known more for its emphasis on the social, language-driven nature of cognitive development and its emphasis upon the cultural and interpersonal levels at which individuals learn. – While Piaget’s theory is known more for its emphasis on cognitive development in stages. However Vygotsky’s also identified, from research focused upon sorting blocks of different colours and shapes, several stages of concept formation (see above ), which are overlooked.

· Unlike Piaget’s theory, Vygotsky’s theory can explain the influence of social environment, through culture and language, upon cognitive development.

· Schaffer (2004) criticises Vygotsky’s theory for failing to include important emotional factors, such as the frustrations of failure and the joys of success, as well as failing to identify the motivational factors children use to achieve particular goals.

Psychology in the real world: Summary of the Impact that each theory has on education.
Both Piaget’s and Vygotsky’s theories have practical applications in education.

Piaget’s theory – contributed the following:

  1. The concept of readiness, which believes children should only be taught things that reflect their level of cognitive development.
  2. The idea of curriculum, which believes certain things should be taught at certain ages, thus reflecting a child’s level of cognitive development.
  3. Discovery learning, which sees children learning by themselves through interaction with their environment.

Therefore, for Piaget the role of a teacher was to assess what level of cognitive development a child was currently at and to provide them with relevant learning.

Vygotsky’s theory – and zone of proximal development clearly links to education through its capacity for teachers, as knowledgeable others, to guide children from what they can currently do to what they can do with help and thus will be able to do by themselves in the future.

Vygotsky’s theory also contributed to collaborative learning, where children of similar levels of ability to work together, promoting the use of language to bring about cognitive development, with knowledge being socially constructed by working collectively on common tasks.

Therefore the role of the teacher is to guide children by mentoring, scaffolding them on to ever greater competence.

Unlike Piaget’s theory, Vygotsky’s theory can explain the influence of social environment, through culture and language, upon cognitive development.

Vygotsky’s ideas have also been applied to education. Child-centred learning must consider a child’s ZPD if sensitive intervention is to be applied effectively.

17
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Explain Selman’s theory perspective taking.

A

Selman’s Levels of PerspectiveTaking:

Robert Selman gave children scenarios along with questions that required them to take the perspective of others. Based on their answers, he developed a stage theory to explain the way in which children develop their ability to take different perspectives. He proposed five stages or levels: undifferentiated perspective-taking; social-informational perspective-taking; self-reflective perspective-taking; mutual perspective-taking; and societalperspective-taking.

18
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Further Explain Selman’s levels of perspective-taking.

A

Selman’s levels of perspective-taking.

In the last three topics we have been looking at how cognitive abilities develop in children. We have seen that both Piaget and Vygotsky described this development in terms of stages: children are born with some innate abilities which are then developed as the child experiences the world. However, just as children have to make sense of their physical world, they also have to come to terms with and understand their social world. This is calledsocial cognition, and the development of social cognition is the focus of this final topic.

Social cognition.

Let’s start by revisiting what Piaget and Vygotsky had to say about the role of social interaction in cognitive development.

Social cognition overview:

Social cognition refersto the unique processes that enable human beings to interpret social information and behave appropriately in a social environment.

As in other domains of cognition, social information processing relies initially on attending to and perceiving relevant cues. While some debate exists as to what is included under the term social cognition, this wide-ranging construct is often conceptualised as consisting of four core domains: emotion processing, social processing, metallizing, and attribution style/bias.

Selman and perspective-taking:

Social Cognition is involved with explaining how individuals develop the ability to make sense of their social world. A major element in this is the development of perspective-taking, as documented in Selmen’s levels of perspective-taking, where individuals gradually come to understand the viewpoints of others and understand their thoughts and feelings.

Selman (1980) gave children dilemmas to ponder. For example, Holly had promised her father she would not climb trees after falling out of one, but should she climb a tree to rescue a kitten that is trapped in its branches?

The answers to such dilemmas enabled Selman to devise a theory about the development ofperspective-taking, or the ability to understand things from another person’s point of view. What was important in the children’s answers wasn’t so much whether they would or wouldn’t do something, but thereasoningthey gave behind their decision.

Selman came up with five stages:

Stage 0: Egocentric (undifferentiated) viewpoint (3–6 years)

Stage 1: Social informational role-taking (6–8 years)

Stage 2: Self-reflective role-taking (8–10 years)

Stage 3: Mutual (third-party) role-taking (10–12 years)

Stage 4: Social and conventional system role-taking (12–15+ years).

Example of Selman’s study of Holly and the Kitten:

Very young children do not appreciate that other people have experiences and feelings different to their own, but Selman devised risk-taking theory to explain the development of perspective-taking, where adopting the perspectives of others allows an individual to comprehend their feelings, thoughts and intentions.

The theory was developed through research involving interpersonal dilemmas – such as with 8 year old ‘expert tree-climber Holly, who one day falls out of a tree, which leads to her promising her father she will not climb trees any more.

However, Holly and her friends then go to meet another friend, whose kitten is stuck in a tree and may fall at any moment. Only Holly has the climbing skills to save the kitten. Should she climb the tree to save the kitten?

  • The theory has five levels, developed from children’s answers to questions about such dilemmas. As children mature, they are seen to take more information into account, coming to realise that people can react differently to the same situation. They develop the ability to analyse people’s perspectives from the viewpoint of an objective, neutral bystander, and come to realise how different cultural and societal values affect the perception of the bystander. (See the different stages highlighted in the digital textbook, with each giving an example of Holly’s behaviour).

Summary of Selman’s 5 stages (use for just referencing, and remember to keep in mind the depth highlighted above):

Stage 0: Egocentric (undifferentiated) viewpoint (3–6 years) – children are unaware of any perspective other than their own.

Stage 1: Social informational role-taking (6–8 years) – children now recognize that people can have perspectives that differ from their own but believe that this happens only because these individuals have received different information.

Stage 2: Self-reflective role-taking (8–10 years) – children now know that their own and others’ points of view may conflict even if they have received the same information. They are now able to consider the other person’s viewpoint.

Stage 3: Mutual (third-party) role-taking (10–12 years) – the child can now simultaneously consider her own and another person’s points of view and recognise that the other person can do the same. The child can also assume the perspective of a disinterested third party and anticipate how each participant will react to the viewpoint of his or her partner.

Stage 4: Social and conventional system role-taking (12–15+ years) – the adolescent now attempts to understand another person’s perspective by comparing it with that if the social system in which he or she operates (i.e., the view of the “ generalized other”). In other words, the adolescent expects others to consider and typically assume perspectives on events that most people in his or her social group would take.

19
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Provide research evidence for Selmans levels of perspective-taking.

A

Many pieces of research have been carried out to demonstrate Selman’s five stages

Summary of studies:

Selman (1971) gave children information that a child in a scenario did not have and asked them to predict the child’s behaviour.

Selman and Byrne (1974) presented children with two interpersonal dilemmas and asked them to discuss the perspectives of the different people in the dilemmas.

Gurucharri and Selman (1982) carried out alongitudinal studyusing Selman’s own methodology and dilemmas.

Schultz and Selman (1990) looked at the transition from self-centred to the development of an ability to perceive other people’s perspectives.

Epleyet al(2004) assessed egocentric bias in children and adults.

Research in depth:

(1) Selman (1971) – Methodology: Got children aged four to six years to predict a child’s behaviour after being given information about a situation that was not available to the child in question. Outcomes: Participants, especially the younger ones, tended to make a prediction based on the information they had been given. What this shows: This suggests they were in the egocentric viewpoint stage in line with Selman’s theory, as they could not see the situation from the child’s perspective.

(2) Selman & Byrne (1974) – Methodology: Presented children aged between four and ten with two interpersonal dilemmas and then in interviews got them to discuss the perspectives of the different characters involved in each dilemma. Outcomes: Children aged four to six years tended to show evidence of having an egocentric viewpoint, perceiving things from their own perspective, while children aged four to six years tended to show evidence of being in the social informational role-taking stage, understanding that people have different viewpoints, but were able to consider only viewpoint at a time. What this shows: Children aged eight to ten were increasingly able to see things from different people’s perspectives. The results support Selman’s theory, as they imply that perspective-taking increases with age in set stages.

20
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Briefly evaluate Selman’s levels - Ao3.

A

Selman’s theory (as with Piaget’s) has had practical consequences for education. For instance, there is little point in teaching team sports until children can understand another’s point of view. Also, once someone can empathise with another person’s feelings it can be used to help with managing aggression.
Selman has been criticised for ignoring non-cognitive factors, such as the role that arguments and mediation play in promoting perspective-taking skills. There is also a view that he may have focused too heavily on western cultures and that the findings of his dilemmas may not be applicable to other cultures.

Evaluative points on Selman’s theory of perspective-taking – and parallels between Selman and Piaget:

Selman & Piaget:

· There are parallels between Selman’s theory and Piaget’s cognitive development theory. – Egocentrism is a central feature of Piaget’s idea of pre-operational thinking (see page 3 in textbook) and forms the basis of Selman’s egocentric viewpoint, while decentring, the ability to perceive the world from more one perspective, is a central figure of Piaget’s idea of operational thought and of Selman’s later stages.

In general:
- Selman’s stages 1 and 2 are seen as relating to Piaget’s pre-operational stage.

  • Stages 3 or 4 to his concrete operational stage.
  • And stages 4 or 5 to his formal operational stage.

Criticisms:

· Selman’s theory has been criticised for focusing too much on the effect of cognitive development on perspective-taking and social cognition and downplaying the role of non-cognitive factors. For example, social factors such as arguments between friends have been seen to promote perspective-taking skills, and mediation from others in settling disagreements plays a similar role.

· Research into perspective-taking may actually be culturally biased, as research was carried out mainly on children from Western cultural backgrounds and therefore findings may not be applicable or appropriate to children from other cultures. Quintana et al. (1999) – criticised Selman’s work as disregarding the development of perspective-taking in ethnic sub-cultural groupings.

Strengths:

· The developmental claims of Selman’s model are supported by research evidence – individuals progress gradually to higher stages over time, with little evidence of any regression to lower stages.

· Selman’s use of interpersonal dilemmas has provided researchers with an objective means of assessing social competence that has become a paradigm (accepted) method of studying the development of perspective-taking.

· Perspective-taking has practical applications as a means of conflict resolution. Walker & Selman (1998) – used perspective-taking to reduce aggression levels by getting individuals to empathise with other people’s feelings and viewpoints.

· Selman’s theory has a practical application in physical education, as it has been used to ascertain the ages at which children can understand others’ viewpoints and roles within competitive team sports. There is little point in trying to teach sports to children before they are less egocentric and can appreciate other’s viewpoints.

21
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Explain the theory of mind (ToM) and the Sally-Anne test.

A

Theory of Mind isthe branch of cognitive science that investigates how we ascribe mental states to other persons and how we use the states to explain and predict the actions of those other persons. More accurately, it is the branch that investigates mindreading or mentalizing or mentalistic abilities.

Theory of mind(ToM)is a phrase that was first used to describe the cognitive ability of chimpanzees. Found only in species of animals with high intelligence, ToM is used to describe the understanding of the thoughts and emotions of other people. An important moment in the development of a mind is to realise that other people also have a mind with its own feelings and emotions. This ability is not present at birth but develops over time.

As with Selman’s work, ToM was investigated by presenting children with scenarios and asking them to interpret the actions of characters within them. The most famous scenario involved the ability to attribute false beliefs to others.

One of the most famous ‘false belief tasks’ is the Sally–Anne test which involved two dolls, one called Sally and one called Anne.

When the test is carried out it is important to make sure that the children observing the task can distinguish between the two dolls and correctly observe them. The scenario is basically as follows:

Sally places a marble in a basket and leaves the stage.

Anne comes along and transfers the marble, hiding it in a box.

Sally returns and the child is asked where Sally will look for the marble.

The correct answer is that Sally will still think the marble is in the basket and so will look there. If the child answers ‘in the box‘, then they have not acquired the ability to see that Sally has a mind which contains different information from their own mind.

  • ToM is similar to Piaget’s idea of egocentrism, with ToM not developing until decentring (see page 6 in digital textbook) occurs, at around 4 years of age. A more recent view emphasis modularity, where specific brain areas, such as the amygdala and basal ganglia, are associated with ToM processing, with a set sequence of development occurring and with ToM reasoning being inferred from other knowledge.
22
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Explain the theory of mind (ToM) in relation to in explaining autism.

A

Role in explaining autism:

Autism.

When you made notes on the Sally–Anne study you will have seen that it has been used to explore autism, which has been likened to ’mind-blindness’. People with autism have problems in communication and building relationships, which may be due to their inability to appreciate that other people have a mind that is unique to them.

People with autism do not necessarily suffer from other cognitive deficits, and in fact can exhibit highly advanced cognitive abilities. Simon Baron-Cohen (1993) explored how people with autism could display an inability to see things from another person’s perspective but still possess other cognitive functioning.

Leslie (1987) proposed a theory of mind mechanism (ToMM), suggesting that ToM is innate but requires a biological maturation process. If physiological damage occurs before or after birth, this maturation can be prevented from occurring.

This could then result in the cognitive deficits shown in autism.
Problems in specific areas of the brain, such as the left medial prefrontal cortex, have been identified as being involved in autistic deficits using PET scans. There is also evidence, from studying activity in the fusiform gyrus area of the brain, that people with autism experience problems in processing facial information.

The lack of eye gaze has been shown to be a better measure of social deficits in children with autism. It was found that children with learning deficits, but not those with autism, would look in the correct place when a marble was hidden in a third location in an adaptation of the Sally–Anne test.

Research support studies for autism and ToM:

  • If a child makes the false-belief error in the Sally–Anne test then they have not understood that another person can have a mind which is different from theirs, and so are suffering from ’mind-blindness’, which is characteristic of autism.
  • Tager-Flusberg (2007) – reviewed false belief task studies done with children with to find that in all studies there were examples of children with autism who passed false belief tasks, which therefore casts doubts on ToM being a complete explanation for autism.
  • Frith (1989) – was the first to suggest that children with autism may not be able to understand that other people, and indeed themselves, have a ‘mind’. Frith argued that autism was associated with mind-blindness, an inability to understand what other people were thinking and feeling.
23
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: evaluate ToM explanation for explaining - A03.

A

Evaluation of autism and ToM.

A lack of ToM appears to be a plausible explanation for many of the symptoms of autism, such as the problems with communication. It can also explain why there is an inability to engage in pretend play, where it is crucial to be able to reflect on one’s own thoughts.

However, there are doubts as to whether a lack of ToM would actually leave other cognitive abilities intact. Some people with autism display exceptional mathematical skills, and this should not be the case if a lack of ToM affected other cognitive skills.

Children with autism display social deficits in the first year of life, even if they can later pass the false-belief test. ToM is unable to explain these social deficits, such as the appropriate response to vocal and facial expressions.

There are doubts as to whether ToM is universal or specific to autism. Such doubts are based on the evidence that some children with autism can pass ToM tests while some with specific language impairments fail them. It is possible to improve performance on ToM tests with tutoring, and this throws doubt on the innate status of ToM.

There are also doubts about assigning a specific brain region to ToM.

Evaluative points of ToM as an explanation of autism:

Cristisms:

· A problem with using ToM as an explanation for autism is that it would be expected that the apparent problems children with autism have in reflecting on their own thoughts would affect their ability to perform complex cognitive actions, such as solving mathematical problems, but some children with autism have advanced mathematical skills.

· If ToM was the main explanation for autism, three predictions would be met:
1. That children with autism fail tests of ToM.
2. That ToM is innate.
3. That ToM relates to a specific brain region.

However, many children with autism pass ToM tests, while children who do not have autism but who have specific language impairments fail them, so ToM is neither universal in nor specific to autism. Also, tutoring in mind-reading and grammar improves performance on ToM tests, suggesting it is not innate, while brain scanning studies have failed to isolate a specific brain region.

· Even when children with autism pass false belief tasks, they often perform poorly in tasks requiring use of social stimuli. Even in the first year of life social interactions require recognition of and appropriate responses to vocal and facial expressions and children with autism display early evidence of being unable to do this. ToM is unable to explain such social deficits.

Strengths:

· A lack of ToM provides plausible reasons for many symptoms of autism. For example, not being able to understand other people’s thoughts could be seen to lead to the difficulties children with autism often have with communicating via language.

· Another instance in the lack of ToM can also explain the lack of pretend play exhibited by children with autism, through children with autism not being able reflect on their own thoughts. When a child pretends to be a mother to a doll, the child must be capable of simultaneously holding in their mind two contradictory sets of beliefs, the child knows it is a child and that the doll isn’t real, but must think the opposite – that they are a mother and the doll is a baby. Without ToM it is not possible to do this, as children often with autism demonstrate.

24
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Describe the Sally-Anne test in depth.

A

The classic experiment:

Beginning in1983, a series of studies used fictional scenarios to try and assess at what age children start to get a grasp of the existence of false beliefs. Perhaps the most influential of these experiments is known asthe Sally Anne task, developed by Simon Baron-Cohen, Alan Leslie and Uta Frith, then at the MRC cognitive development unit in London. In the experiment, children were presented with two dolls, Sally (who has a basket) and Anne (who has a box). Sally puts a marble in her basket, and leaves the room. While Sally is away, Anne takes the marble from the basket, and hides it in her box. Finally, Sally returns to the room, and the child is asked three questions:

Where will Sally look for her marble? (The “belief” question)

Where is the marble really? (The “reality” question)

Where was the marble at the beginning? (The “memory” question)

The critical question is the belief question – if children answer this by pointing to the basket, then they have shown an appreciation that Sally’s understanding of the world doesn’t reflect the actual state of affairs. If they instead point to the box, then they fail the task, arguably because they haven’t taken into account that they possess knowledge that Sally doesn’t have access to. The reality and memory questions essentially serve as control conditions; if either of these are answered incorrectly, then it might suggest that the child didn’t quite understand what was going on.

In the original study by Baron-Cohen and colleagues, three groups of children were tested: typically developing children (aged around four), children with Down’s syndrome (aged around 11), and children with autism (aged around 12). The findings were comparable for the typically developing children and those with Down’s syndrome – in both groups, around 85% of participants correctly answered the belief question.

Research support studies for ToM:

  • Flavell et al. (1986) – found that three year olds who handled a sponge that like a rock called it a rock, while four year olds called it a sponge, suggesting that the development of a ToM requires an appreciation of what is false and that a lack of ToM is a cognitive deficit. (Study summarised above).
  • Harris (1989) – (with support to Flavell’s study) reported that at around the age of four, children become aware of their emotions and use them to pretend to be someone else, permitting them to be aware of others’ thinking, suggesting that this is a pivotal age in reading that others think differently.

Evaluation of ToM:

Some criticisms of the work on ToM have been based on the language used and on the complex nature of some of the tasks. As with Piaget’s work, it is suggested that the children in the studies might have misunderstood the questions used, based on their limited language skills, and did actually have a ToM. They also might have been fazed by the complex nature of some of the tasks.

A lack of ToM is similar to Piaget’s idea onegocentrismand so the two concepts might be linked. ToM appears at about the age of 4, but there is a question as to whether this happens suddenly or whether there is a gradual development from about the age of 2.

The involvement of particular regions of the brain, and the fact that this occurs at the same age in various cultures, indicates that the process is one of biological maturation and not of learning.

Study hint:

Note that it is important to differentiate between perceptual perspective-taking (seeing another perspective, e.g. the ’three mountains’ task) and conceptual perspective-taking (understanding another belief, e.g. Sally–Anne).
Studies:

Perceptual (the children’s perspectives) perspective-taking = e.g. ‘three mountains’ task on egocentrism.

Conceptual (the children’s understanding of the concept) perspective-taking = e.g. ‘Sally-Anne’ study.

25
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Explain the role of the mirror neuron system in social cognition.

A

The role of the mirror neuron system in social cognition.

Professor Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues (1996). They noticed that neurons in the premotor cortex of monkeys’ brains fired when the monkeys saw the researchers reaching for food. This brain area was the same area that fired when the monkeys themselves moved their hands. It seemed that the monkeys’ brains were acting as if their own hand was moving when, instead, they were seeing someone else’s hand moving.

The mirror neuron system:

The discovery of mirror neurons has led to the idea that we can understand other people’s feelings not just by logical thought processes but by actually creating the self-same feelings inside ourselves. The phrase ’I feel for you’, which you might say when you empathise with someone, is now seen as being literally true.

Further research has shown that mirror neurons can distinguish not only between different actions but between different intentions of an action. One famous piece of work showed different areas of the brain firing when watching someone pick up a cup to drink from as opposed to those which fired when the person picked up the cup to clear it away.

Defective mirror neuron systems could explain why people with autism can’t empathise with another person’s feelings.

The role of the mirror neuron system in social cognition:

Mirror neurons are nerves in the brain that are active when specific actions are performed or observed in others, therefore allowing observers to experience the action as if it were their own. Mirror neurons may permit individuals to share in the feelings and thoughts of others by emphasising with and imitating others, and thus allow them to have a ToM.

The action of mirror neurons is such that when individuals experience an emotion such as disgust, or view an expression of disgust on another person’s face, the same mirror neurons are activated. This allows an observer and the person being observed to have direct experimental understanding of each other, thus explaining how people empathise with each other (understand each other’s feelings).

· Mirror neurons are found in brain areas involved in social cognition, especially water-related areas (areas concerned with planning and executing movement), such as the inferior frontal, premature and inferior partial cortices brain areas.

· Mirror neurons may be the biological mechanism by which the understanding of our own and the mental states of others occurs, and defective mirror neuron systems could explain conditions of social communication and interaction deficiencies, such as autism.

Research:

Gallese (2001) – used fMRI scans to show that the anterior cingulate cortex and inferior frontal cortex are active when individuals experience emotion or observe another person experiencing the same emotion, supporting the idea of mirror neuron-type activity occurring in humans.

Evaluation of mirror neurons.

Much of mirror neuron research has been carried out on monkeys; ethically and practically it is impossible to carry out this sort of work on humans.

There is therefore a question of validity in generalising from monkey research to humans. However, recent studies using fMRI scans have found evidence of mirror neurons in the same areas of the human brain as in monkeys.

Mirror neuron systems have been discovered in arangeof animals which exhibit complex social groupings. This would indicate that they have evolved as a result of their survival value. However, recently they have been discovered in birds and this might imply that the ability is widespread in the animal kingdom.

Heyes (2012) raises the issue of cause and effect. Does the evolution of mirror neurons cause us to understand each other’s actions, or are they a consequence of social interactions between humans?

The research into mirror neurons has given us a biological explanation for autism; however, the evidence is far from conclusive as yet. Research (see below) by Daprettoet al(2006) has explored mirror neurons in children suffering from autism compared with normal children.

Evaluation points on the theory behind mirror neurons:

Criticisms:

· For practical reasons there is still a methodological problem in studying mirror neurons in humans, as it is not possible to study the actions of single neurons. Therefore it is arguable as to whether the findings from studies on macaque monkeys can be generalised to humans – for example, adult monkeys cannot learn by imitation, while human adults can.

· Social cognition seems to exist only in species of animals that live in fairly complex social groupings, with indications of mirror neuron systems being found in primates, elephants and dogs, which suggests a biological basis of social cognition that has evolved due its adaptive survival value.

However Atkins et al. (2002) – found evidence of a mirroring system in birds, namely Japanese quail, which implies the ability may be more widespread in the animal kingdom.

Strengths:

· Studies using fMRI scans have found evidence of mirror neurons systems in humans and in the same areas as it the monkey, though fMRI scans also suggest a wider network of brain areas is involved, including the somosensory Cortext, which may help an observer perceive what it feels like to perform a certain action.

· Research into mirror neurons has indicated a possible biological explanation for autism, and greater understanding may pave the way for developing methods of counteracting the social deficits associated with the disorder, although evidence linking autism and defective motor neuron systems is far form conclusive as yet.

Contemporary research: Understanding the emotions in others – Dapretto et al (1985).

Mirror neurons may allow observers to directly experience the behaviours of others, which could explain how humans empathise with each other. Dapretto and her colleagues were interested in seeing whether specific brain areas are related to mirror neuron ability. As children with autism often lack the ability to empathise with others – their brain activity was compared with that of normally developed children to see whether specific brain areas could be identified.

Their aim:

To examine mirror neuron ability in children with autism and normally developed using fMRI scanning.

Their produce:

  • Participants were ten high-functioning children with autism and ten normally functioning children aged between 10 and 14 years.
  • Participants and their parents gave informed consent in line with Ethics Review Board of the University of California.
  • Eighty facial expressions representing five different emotions – anger, fear, happiness, neutrality and sadness – were presented for two seconds each in a random sequence.
  • fMRI scans were used as participants either observed or initiated the faces presented (Counterbalanced within each group).

Their findings:

· Both groups of children observed the stimuli and imitated the facial expressions. However, children with autism showed no mirror neuron activity in the inferior frontal gyrus brain region.

· The relationship between activity in mirror neuron brain areas and symptom severity of the children with autism was examined, using scores on the Autism Diagnostic interview – Revised Scale. A negative correlation was found between activity in the parts operculrasis brain area and scores of the children with autism.

· Activity in the insula and Limbic structures, brain areas underlying emotional understanding was also negatively correlated with symptom severity.

Their conclusions:

There are differences in the neutral pathways used by typically developing children and children with autism.

Typically developing children rely on a right hemisphere mirror neuron mechanism, which interfaces with the limbic system via the insula brain area, whereby the meaning of an observed emotion is directly understood.

In children with this mirroring mechanism is not engaged, thus the emotional significance of observed emotions is not understood.

Mirror neurons underlie the ability to read other’s emotional states from facial expressions.

Evaluation points:

· The lack of mirror neuron activity during the observation and imitation of emotional expressions in children with autism provides support for the idea that early dysfunction on the mirror system is a key factor of the social deficits seen in autism.

· The research suggests a biological foundation to the development of social cognition in humans and also to the development of autism.

26
Q

PC: T4. Selman and the development of social cognition: Evaluate the development of social cognition explanation.

A

Summary of The development of social cognition:

In this topic you have seen how Selman explored perspective-taking using interpersonal dilemmas. He described five stages that children pass through, from seeing situations from their own perspective to understanding that others might be affected by larger societal views.

Theory of mind (ToM) explores the idea that humans can develop the ability to attribute mental states to themselves and to others. This has been used to explain autism, by suggesting that suffers are experiencing ‘mind-blindness’ due to the inability to acquire ToM.

The Sally–Anne test has been used to assess ToM along with alternative ‘false-belief’ tasks.

A biological explanation of ToM has been suggested using evidence that some neurons fire in the brain when an action is observed as well as when the individual is carrying out the action themselves.