Module 1: Incidence and prevalence Flashcards
PECOT
- Population: group of people who share a
specified common factor. - Exposure group
- Comparison group
- Outcomes:
• EGO: occurrence of dis-ease in exposure
group.
• CGO: occurrence of dis-ease in comparison
group. - An average can be taken and EG
compared to CG.
Incidence
- Incidence: counting the number of onsets of disease events occurring during a period of time.
• Longitudinal measure producing a rate.
• Most appropriate for observable events.
• Require dis-ease outcome to be categorical
variable.
• Measuring prevalence at two points of time
and calculating the change in prevalence bt/
the two points in time is a measure of the
incidence of dis-ease over the period bt/ the
two time points
ADVANTAGES
- Most useful for measuring causes of dis-ease
occurrence.
- Incidence is determined only by the dis-ease
risk in the population.
- Measures of incidence include events,
population and time.
DISADVANTAGES
- Incidence can be difficult to measure as one has
to observe events over time.
Prevalence
Prevalence: counting the number of people w/ a
dis-ease at a point in time.
• Cross sectional measure producing a figure.
• Most appropriate when transition from a nondis-eased state to a dis-eased state cannot
easily be observed and counted.
ADVANTAGES
- Prevalence is relatively easy to measure, as it is
static, taken for one point in time.
- Useful to funders and planners of health.
DISADVANTAGES
- Prevalence measures only include events and
population.
- Prevalence is determined by incidence, death
rate and cure rate
Prevalence
POINT PREVALENCE
- Point prevalence: outcome does not take any
previous time period into account and is simply
measured at one point in time.
PERIOD PREVALENCE
- Period prevalence: outcome/numerator depends
on the time period specified.
• Dis-ease outcomes cannot easily be
measured at one point in time, so we look
back and measure them over a period of
time.
A population could have a high incidence/low
prevalence if death/cure rate is also high.
A population could have a low incidence/high
prevalence if death/cure rate is also low.
C o m p a r i s o n s — R i s k
Differences and Relative Risk
- Differences bt/ EGO and CGO can provide
insight into the size of the effect of the study
exposure on the dis-ease outcome. - Comparisons of dis-ease occurrence typically
called ‘estimates of effect/association’ of an
exposure on a dis-ease outcome.
RISK DIFFERENCE
- EGO-CGO
- Units — same unit as EGO/CGO calculation e.g.
per x people over y years — more info. - Risk ratio can be the same whereas risk
difference can be much smaller/larger. - Also called difference in occurrences, absolute
risk (difference). - RD=0, no difference in effect of E and C on the
study outcome. - Risk difference is an absolute risk reduction if
risk is lower in the exposure group or an
absolute risk increase if the risk is higher in the
exposure group.
RELATIVE RISK
- EGO/CGO
- Also called risk ratio, relative risk difference,
ratio of occurrence. - No units — less information.
- RR=1, ‘no-effect’ value.
- If dis-ease occurrence measures are calculated
as averages, relative comparison of two mean
scores is ‘relative mean’ (RM). - Relative risk reduction: relative risk is subtracted
from 1.0, then expressed as a percentage. - Relative risk increase: 1.0 subtracted from
relative risk, then express as percentage.
Non Random Error
- Also called biases/systematic errors.
- If error occurs because of poor study design,
processes or measurement.
• Valid study: small amount of random/nonrandom error.
RECRUITMENT -RAMBOMAN
- Are participants representative sample from a
defined population? - Described as external validity error, as when
present findings may not be applicable to wider
population. - Particularly important when major objective of
study to measure characteristics of real population but participants recruited not
representative of eligibles.
Selection bias: participants allocated to EG
different source to participants allocated to CG.
• Confounding error caused by allocation
process.
- Non response bias: non-responders different to
responders. Consider response rate.
ALLOCATION
How well were participants allocated to EG and
CG?
- Confounders: EG and CG differ in ways other
than allocation, and these other difference have
an effect on the study outcome.
- RCTs: allocation by random process; all
participants have equal chance of allocation to
EG or CG, so groups are similar.
• RCTs are known as experiments, because
investigators actively control allocation
process.
• RCTs best way to stop confounding. - Complete baseline comparison to ensure
RCTs w/ small samples don’t have different
groups just through chance alone
Concealment of allocation stops tampering w/ randomisation process. - Observational studies: allocate by measurement and assign to EG and CG accordingly. • People may lie/under-report to hide embarrassment or they can’t remember. - Avoid by good questionnaire design. • Inaccurate measurement of exposure: (allocation) measurement error. • CG and EG often quite different: confounders if study outcome influenced. - Adjust for in analyses. - Sufficient information must be collected about other differences for adjustment purposes. - Confounding present in almost every observational study. • Two or more effects mix, all
MAINTENANCE
Will the validity of the study results be affected
by how well they were maintained in EG and
CG?
- Maintenance error: some participants’ exposure
status changes, or some are lost to follow-up.
• Did participants remain in their allocated
groups? Did they maintain their initial
exposure/comparison exposure?
- Long term cohort studies prone to maintenance
bias — offset by regular follow-ups.
- Maintenance not a problem for cross-sectional
studies.
BLIND AND OBJECTIVE MEASUREMENT
• Reduce error by blinding participants and
investigators to knowledge of which
intervention participants received.
• Blinding of outcome measurement ideal for
death certification when pathologist blind.
• R e d u c e e r r o r b y t a k i n g o b j e c t i v e
measurements where possible e.g. using a
machine.
- Measurement: use a standard definition.
ANALYSES
- Confounding can be reduced by dividing
participants into strata — stratified analysis e.g.
age standardisation.
Random Error
- Due to chance.
- No single study will ever measure the exact truth
in the whole population, even if it is a perfect
study.
• Every study an ‘estimate of the truth’.
• Identical studies will produce different results. - All measures of EGO/CGO/RR/RD/NNT have
random error.
• Most random errors can be reduced by
i n c re a s i n g s a m p l e s i z e , re p e a t i n g
measurements. - Extreme events are often chance events:
repeating measurements or studies w/ extreme
results many times usually gives less extreme
results — regression to the mean.
RANDOM SAMPLING ERROR
- Inherent in every study.
• N o s a m p l e w i l l e v e r b e p e r f e c t l y
representative of the population. - Every sample will differ due to chance, and
never include participants w/ identical
characteristics. - Bigger sample, smaller differences bt/ sample
and population
RANDOM MEASUREMENT/ASSESSMENT ERROR
- R a n d o m m e a s u r e m e n t e r r o r e ff e c t s
measurement of both exposures and outcomes. - Ability to measure biological factors the same
way every time is often poor, particularly when a
human operator is involved.
• Avoid by repeated measurements w/ an
average, or use, automatic, objective
machine. - Randomness inherent in biological phenomena.
• Inherent variability in biological phenomena,
and therefore in its measurement.
• Identical measurements of exposures and
outcomes in the same or similar people can
change from moment to moment.
RANDOM ALLOCATION ERROR
- Groups in an RCT may differ by chance alone,
particularly if trial is small. - Reduce by undertaking a larger study.
CONFIDENCE INTERVALS
- Every epidemiological measure has random
error in the estimate of the truth (EGO, CGO, RR,
RD) in the population that the study participants
were recruited from which can be estimated by a
confidence interval. - 95% CI definition: in 100 identical studies using
samples from the same population, 95/100 of
the 95% CIs will include the true value for the
population.
• There is about a 95% chance that the true
value in the population from which the
participants were recruited lies within the
95% CI, assuming no random error.
• Bigger the study, narrower the CI. - As number of events in study increases, recruited. Probably no statistically significant difference
• 95% CIs for RR and RD usually cross no effect line.
• Study results not statistically significant.
• If 95% CIs of RR and RD cross no effect line,
best stated as too much random error to
determine if there is a difference bt/ EGO and
CGO, as opposed to stating there is no
statistically significant difference. - 95% CI just touches no-effect line, study
“borderline statistically significant” (compare w/
statistically/not statistically significant). - Statistically significant event may be clinically
significant if a clinician would make a similar
decision whether the true result was near one
end of the confidence interval or the other.
Similarly, a small but statistically significant
effect w/ a narrow confidence interval may not
be of clinical significance.
width of 95% CIs decrease.
• Wider the interval, more random error in
measure.
• 95% CI most common for epidemiological
studies.
• 99% CI wider than a 95% CI.
• Confidence limit: each end of CI. - If no overlap of confidence intervals, reasonable
to assume that EGO and CGO are truly different
from each other in the underlying population.
• When there is no overlap of CIs in EGO and
CGO, confidence intervals for RD and RR will
not cross no-effect line.
• Therefore measures of association bt/ EGO
and CGO show real effect and ‘study results
are statistically significant’. - If overlap of CIs, study unable to determine if
EGO is different from CGO in the population
from which the study participants were