Lecture 6: Language Production Flashcards
1
Q
stages of speech production (6)
A
- 1) Conceptualizing the overall meaning we want to express
- 2) Formulating what words to use, and what order to put them in.
- Lexical access: select words to express these concepts.
- Organize words syntactically to convey a message.
- 3) Articulation: Implement each word as a series of sounds that comes out of our mouths.
- 4) Monitoring: Repair/stop and restart/utter editing expression (e.g. I’m sorry, I mean).
2
Q
disfluencies in speech planning (5)
A
- Many types of disfluencies: pauses, breaks, false starts, repairs, repetitions, fillers (e.g. uh, uhm), prolongations, etc.
- Aren’t randomly distributed; occur under heavy cognitive load.
- e.g. 1) Planning a new utterance, 2) describing something difficult, 3) distraction, 4) before low frequency words, 5) before words that have more alternatives, 6) before objects that don’t have a conventional name, etc.
- Listeners are sensitive to speakers’ difficulty.
- e.g. Participants shown an ice cream cone and a weird shape, then hear Click on the uh… → look more to the weird shape that’s harder to describe.
3
Q
What do speech errors tell us about production? (2)
A
- What the planning units are.
- When different parts are being planned.
4
Q
word-level speech errors (6)
A
- substitution; e.g. I put the mushrooms in the water. (I put the flowers in the water.)
- exchange; e.g. write a mother to my letter
- anticipation; e.g. sky is in the sky
- Errors almost always involve two words of the same syntactic category.
- e.g. Once I stop, I can’t start. You would insert a noun here.
- They also tend to involve two words that are semantically connected.
5
Q
morpheme-level speech errors (3)
A
- When we insert morphemes in the wrong place.
- addition; e.g. wanted to strained it.
- shift; e.g. I want to readed (I wanted to read)
6
Q
phoneme-level speech errors (2)
A
- perseveration; beef needle (beef noodle)
- Happens most with long vowels.
7
Q
lexical bias (1)
A
- The statistical tendency for sound-based speech errors to result in actual words rather than nonwords.
8
Q
Baars, Motley, & McKay (1975) (4)
A
- Had subjects read word pairs.
- darn boore → barn door
- dart board → bart board
- Were more likely to produce the first error.
- Demonstrated the lexical bias.
9
Q
types of word-level errors (4)
A
- phonological errors; e.g. cat → mat
- semantic errors; e.g. cat → dog
-
‘mixed’ errors; e.g. cat → rat
- More likely than cat → mat and cat → dog together.
10
Q
How is the lexical entry accessed? (3)
A
- Option 1: The whole lexical entry is accessed; bag of marbles metaphor: you just pick everything out of a bag of marbles.
- Option 2: We access meaning first, sound second.
- What we see evidence for.
11
Q
two-step model (1)
A
- Message –Lexicon→ Lemma Level (Grammatical Encoding) → Lexeme Level (Phonological Encoding) –Lexicon→ Articulation

12
Q
picture-word interference task (7)
A
- Provides evidence for the two-step model.
- Participants see a picture (the target; e.g. a dog) with a word on it (the prime).
- Primes can be: unrelated (e.g. dog-bed), semantically related (e.g.dog-cat), or phonologically related (e.g. dog-doll).
- Dependent variable: latency to name picture.
- Semantic relation → slowdown; interference.
- Phonological relation → facilitation.
- Provides indirect evidence that meaning comes before sound.
13
Q
Meyer (1996) (6)
A
- Study conducted with Dutch participants; had to say sentences with the structure The X is next to Y.
- e.g. The church is next to the arrow.
- Experiment 1: Before hearing target sentence, hear a distractor word that’s semantically related/unrelated to either X or Y.
- Result: Semantic interference; reaction times to both words (X and Y) were slower than when the distractor word was unrelated.
- Experiment 2: Before hearing the target sentence, participants hear a distractor word that’s phonologically related/unrelated to either X or Y.
- Result: Phonological facilitation effects for only the first word; even if the distractor word rhymed with the second word, there was no effect.
- Conclusion: Meaning comes before phonology; retrieval of meaning “lemma” happens while the utterance is being planned, but the planning of sound comes later—while uttering the sentence.
14
Q
What affects the order/structure of the sentence? (5)
A
- The message to be conveyed.
- The grammar of one’s language.
- Subtle shadings of meaning or emphasis.
- e.g. The garden swarmed with bees vs. Bees swarmed in the garden.
- Both of these communicate the same relations, but the first sentence seems to suggest (more than the second) that the garden is mostly filled with bees.
- Lexical selection; heavy-NP shift.
15
Q
heavy-NP shift (1)
A
- A syntactic structure in which a long NP, usually a direct object, is moved toward the end of the sentence instead of in its normal spot adjacent to the verb.
16
Q
Ferreira (1996) (6)
A
- Participants created sentences out of collections of words that were presented on a screen.
- These verbs allowed participants to either create one or two versions of a sentence.
- e.g. For the verb gave: I gave the children the toys or I gave the toys to the children.
- e.g. For the verb donated: I donated the toys to the children. It sounds strange to say I donated the children the toys.
- Participants made fewer errors and spoke faster when they had to create a sentence using the more flexible verb gave.
- Reveals that speakers use syntax as a way of easing the burden of speaking.
17
Q
Brock (1986a) (5)
A
- Participants presented with a picture of a church being struck by lightning and asked to describe the picture.
- Primed with thunder (semantically related to lightning) or worship (semantically related to church).
- If they heard thunder, they were more likely to use the active structure (73% vs. 12%), but if they were primed with worship they were more likely to use the passive structure (20% vs. 65%).
- Lightning is striking the church (active) vs. The church is being struck by lightning (passive).
- Note that there wasn’t a complete reversal; participants were still used the active structure a majority of the time, but the result is still reliable.
18
Q
Why are active sentences more commonly produced? (4)
A
- Frequency? Syntactic complexity? Animacy? Likely the latter.
- Preference for an animate ‘agent’ subject, and an inanimate ‘patient’ object.
- In a passive, the subject is the patient, and often, inanimate.
- We try to avoid this mismatch between syntactic role and semantic role (i.e. we want the subject to be animate and the object to be inanimate).
19
Q
Brock (1986b) (6)
A
- Participants saw a picture and told to describe it. Before that, they heard a sentence and had to repeat it (prime).
- e.g. Prepositional sentence: A rock star sold some cocaine to an agent.
- e.g. Double object sentence: A rock star sold an agent some cocaine.
- Primed with prepositional structure → 48% prepositional, 31% double object.
- Primed with double object structure → 25% prepositional, 53% double object.
- Showed that the double object structure is preferred overall, but there is still an effect of priming.