lecture 3- tactile and haptic perception Flashcards

1
Q

what is tactile perception?

A

Tactile perception refers to the ability of the brain to understand (perceive) the information that comes from the skin, especially the skin of the hands. The hands are used to record sensory information, and then the brain uses this information to guide the hands during an activity.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

sense of touch is..

A

crucial for survival

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

what are the 3 main groups of receptors?

A
  • mechanoreceptors (respond to mechanical stimuli eg pressure (stroking), stretching and vibration)
  • thermoreceptors (respond to temperature)
  • chemoreceptors (respond to certain types of chemicals eg histamine)

note- this one is not certain
- nociceptors, subtypes of chemo-and mechanorecptors? (mediate the perception of pain)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

state- different types of receptors are distributed throughout the skin and respond to different touch-related stimuli and events

A
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

mechanoreceptors: types

A
  • merkel receptor
  • meissners corpuscle
  • pacinian
  • fuffini cylinder
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

merkel receptor:

A
  • small receptors with sharp borders
  • slow adaption
  • pressure fine texture and shape
  • close to skin surface
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

meissners corpuscle:

A
  • small receptors with sharp borders
  • rapid adaptation
  • indentation motion across skin
  • close to skin surface
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

pacinian corpuscle:

A
  • large receptors with diffuse borders
  • rapid adaptation
  • vibration and fine texture
  • deeper in skin
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

ruffini cylinder:

A
  • large receptors with diffuse borders
  • slow adaptation
  • stretching
  • deeper in skin
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Tactile acuity: how to measure I

A
  1. two-point threshold
    - classical measure in early touch research (but vulnerable to cofounds)
    - minimum separation between two points on the skin that is recognised as two
    => smallest discriminable distance between two points (JND)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

mechanoreceptors: short introduction 2

A

DO THIS SLIDE AFTER LISTENING TO LEC

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

tactile acuity: how to measure II

A
  1. grating acuity
    - 2-AFC task: horizontal vs. vertical orientation
    - Acuity as the spacing for which orientation can still be accurately judged (75% correct)
    - More objective measure (i.e., no temporal offset)
    - Thresholds tend to be a bit lower than with 2-point threshold method (fingertip: ~ 1 mm vs. 2-4 mm)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

receptor mechanisms for tactile acuity

A
  • receptor properties determine the perceptual experience when skin is stimulated
  • merkel receptors (close to skin) respond to grooved stimulus patterns => firing of the fibre reflects pattern of grooved stimulus=> signal detail and texture

sensitive body parts have higher receptor desity
=> better tactile acuity is associated with higher density of Merkel receptors

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

neural processing of touch

A
  • But not the whole story: Higher acuity on index finger pad than on pad of little finger
    even though receptor density is identical → cortical mechanisms
  • Size of receptive fields of cortical neurons also determines the tactile acuity
    (discrimination) → cortical neurons representing body parts with higher acuity have smaller receptive fields
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

what is the definition of receptive field

A

the receptive field is a portion of sensory space that can elicit neuronal cortical responses when stimulated

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

effects of sensory training on tactile acuity

A
  • Intense Braille reading can produce
    superior tactile spatial acuity in blind and
    sighted humans (change in cortical
    representation)
  • Tactile acuity declines with age at a rate of
    about 1% per year
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

duplex theory of texture perception I David Katz (1925/1989)- perception of texture depends on 2 cues:

A

1) spatial cues (available to vision and touch): determined by the size, shape and distribution of surface elements (eg., bumps and grooves- eg braille letters)

2) temporal cues (specific to touch):
determined by the rate of vibration as the skin moves across finely texture surfaces (eg sandpaper) => perception/ discrimination of very fine textures require movements

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

texture perception: temporal cues (receptor mechanisms)

A

hollins and Risner (2001):
sensing fine texture through temporal cues is mediated by perception of vibration

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

texture perception: temporal cues

A

hollins and risner (2000):

  • participants struggle to identigy differences between two fine textures in static conditions but improved considerably when they were able to move their fingers over the textures
  • coarse surfaces were equally discriminable in moving and stationary conditions
17
Q

adaption paradigm:

A

(temporarily inactivating different receptor types)

  • Meissner corpuscle (responds to low frequencies)
    =>adapted at 10 Hz (for 6 min)
  • Pacinian corpuscle (responds to high frequencies)
    => adapted at 250 Hz (for 6 min)
  • 2-AFC task: Which texture is finer?
  • After adaption to 250 Hz vibration participants were unable to discriminate the two textures
    → fine texture discrimination depends on Pacinian corpuscles
18
Q

perceiving texture: vision vs touch

A
  • Visually perceived surface texture is
    strongly influenced by illumination!
  • Surfaces appear rougher with decreasing
    illumination angle (no “roughness
    constancy”)
  • Touch involves direct contact with a
    surface – provides a more reliable
    assessment of surface texture than vision
  • For very fine textures touch becomes
    more accurate (higher resolution than
    vision) + access to temporal cues…
19
Q

interim summary: tactile perception

A
  • Four types of mechanoreceptors with differently sized receptive fields
    and different responses to stimulation (slow vs. rapidly adapting) that mediate different types of tactile perception
  • How to measure tactile acuity
  • Mechanisms underlying tactile acuity
  • Duplex theory of texture perception: Tactile perception of coarse textures is determined by spatial cues and perception of fine textures by temporal (vibration) cues
20
Q

haptic perception- mediated by two afferent subsystems:

A

cutaneous/tactile input
- mechano- and thermoreceptors of the skin
- mediates tactual experience

kinaesthetic input
- mechanoreceptors embedded in muscles, tendons and joints
- contributes to the human perception of limb position => where are our body parts

21
Q

state- haptics usually involves active manual exploration

A
22
Q

active vs passive touch (gibson, 1992)

A

gibson, 1962: difference between active and passive tactual experience

23
Q

passive touch (being touch)

A

object is passively moved across the skin of an observer (no voluntary movement)
=> focuses perception on sensory/bodily responses (skin sensation) of the observer to a stimulus

24
Q

active touch (touching objects)

A

observer moves actively with full control over their movements (engages haptic perception)
=> perception focuses on external object properties

25
Q

why is haptic perception complex?

A

is complex as it requires a close interplay between the sensory system (cutaneous sensations), the motor system (moving the fingers and the hand) and the cognitive system (thinking about information provided by sensory and motor system

26
Q

“Where” system: Haptic Space Perception I- what can localisation be referred relative to?

A

a) the sensory organ (where on the skin touch occurs)
=> depends on spatial resolving capacity of the skin

b) the environment (where in space is a stimulus touched ie how do we localise points in space during haptic exploration when vision is unavailable
=> a number of interesting phenomena have been discovered but no coherent theory yet
=> reliable observation: haptic space is anisotropic ie varies in magnitude depending on direction

26
Q

Somatosensory system:
“What” and “Where”” (?)

A
  • Ongoing debate if the touch/somatosensory system can (also) be divided into a
    “what” (perception of surface properties) and “where” (perceptual guidance of
    action, localisation of touch) subsystems (similar as vision, see Lecture 2)

“What”-system:
* Perception of material/surface properties: a) surface texture – e.g., roughness
and smoothness; b) compliance – i.e., deformability under force; c) thermal
quality; d) weight and e) geometric properties – i.e., shape and size

  • Systematic relationship between exploration actions and object properties
    (Lederman & Klatzky, 1987)
27
Q

Manual exploration for Haptic Perception

A

Lederman & Klatzky, 1987

  • 6 most commonly used exploration
    procedures (i.e., stereotyped pattern
    of manual exploration when humans
    are asked to identify object properties
    without vision)

-Exploration procedures human use – tend
to be optimal → provides the most precise
discrimination for the property in question

  • Optimal activation of neural responses
    (e.g., lateral motion for texture enhances
    the response of slow adapting Merkel
    receptors and creates deep vibrations
    activating the Pacinian Corpuscles)
28
Q

“Where” system: Haptic Space Perception II

A

Kappers and Koendering, 1999, 2003, 2007- Task: adjust a test bar so that it feels
“parallel” to the standard bar.

Participants make large
orientation errors in this task
→When judging bar orientation this is related to the orientation of the hand touching the bar (i.e., egocentric frame of reference)

→Orientation of the hand
touching the bar differs depending on the location (i.e., rotated clockwise for
right side and anti-clockwise for left side)

29
Q

affective touch

A
  • Materials elicit different affective responses when we touch them (e.g., pleasant
    feeling of touching a soft toy and unpleasant feeling of touching sandpaper/flour)
  • Relatively new research area not much is known about affective responses to
    touched materials in the relation to perceptual characteristics of those materials
    (usually only a few perceptual variations are included)
  • Perceptual dimensions of touched materials seem to be systematically associated
    with affective responses
  • Understanding affective responses to materials can help to design human-
    machine interfaces (e.g., assistant robots) or safe work environments/tools
  • Study by Drewing and colleagues (2017): 47 solid, fluid and granular materials
    which participants rated according to 32 perceptual and 20 affective attributes.
30
Q

perceptual and affective material perception

A

study by drewing et al. (2017)

  • 6 perceptual dimensions:
    Fluidity, Roughness, Deformability,
    Fibrousness, Heaviness, and Granularity
  • 3 affective dimensions:
    Valence (pleasantness), Arousal (exciting/boring), and Dominance
  • Rougher materials rated as less pleasant, fluid materials associated with higher arousal (less predictable skin stimulation?), heavy materials judged as more dominant, and deformable materials as less dominant (correlations

range of perceptual and affective responses broader than previously assumed

30
Q

from skin to cortex: somatosensory processing

A

Reminder: Two-Point Thresholds

  • Tactile acuity correlates with the density
    of Merkel receptors
  • Areas on the body with high acuity and
    high receptor density are also devoted
    a larger area of processing in the
    brain…
31
Q

interim summary: haptic perception

A
  • Differences between active and passive touch
  • Common exploration procedures for haptic identification (and their
    optimality)
  • Anisotropy of haptic space
  • Affective touch and the relation between perceptual and affective
    material dimensions
32
Q

Somatosensory Cortex (S1)

A
  • S1 is organised in maps corresponding
    to locations on the body (Penfield &
    Rasmussen, 1950)
  • Somatosensory homunculus: areas on
    the body are represented
    disproportionally in the brain (similar
    to magnification in vision)
  • Large areas for hands and lips (most
    sensitive to touch) vs. small areas for
    leg and back (less sensitive)

note
* (Similar topographical representation
in the motor cortex (M1) where
movements of the body are
processed)

33
Q

Somatosensory Cortex (S1) - Plasticity

A
  • Homunculus shows the relative amount of cerebral cortex surface devoted to the processing of certain sensory inputs
  • Importantly, the map is not fixed: Homunculus varies based on individual experiences (shaped by learning)
  • Classic experiments on owl monkeys by Merzenich and colleagues (1987): Intensive stimulation of a skin area (3 months training) causes an expansion of the cortical maps
34
Q

plasticity in humans

A

Evidence from Musicians: Elbert et al. (1995):
Violinists show enlarged somatosensory cortex of regions that receive touch from the tips of the left hand

35
Q

what is the definition of plasticity

A

An adjustment or
adaptation of a cortical
system to environmental
stimuli - or performance
requirements

36
Q

maladaptive plasticity: focal dystonia

A

Definition: Musician’s Cramp
* Neurological disorder
characterised by the loss of
fine motor control of long
practiced skilled movements during instrumental playing

  • The movement disorder is
    task specific (limited to
    instrumental playing)
  • Painless muscular incoordination

Strong correlation between instrument group
and localisation of the focal dystonia (hand
with higher workload becomes affected) +
occurs more often in the dominant hand

37
Q

Maladaptive Plasticity: Focal Dystonia cont

A
  • Primary source of the problem is in the brain not in the peripheral
    nervous system
  • Studies have identified a few key mechanisms related to the disease

– one of them related to plasticity in somatosensory cortex

  • Patients with focal dystonia have abnormalities in the hand/ finger
    representation in the primary sensory cortex
    → Abnormal sensory functions causes issues in motor behaviour
38
Q

Maladaptive Plasticity: Focal Dystonia - bara- jimenez et al. (1998)

A
  • Mapping of finger representations in 6
    patients with focal dystonia
  • Intracortical distance between
    representations of different digits are
    reduced in patients
  • Finger representations are randomly
    organised in patients (usual topography
    inverted)
  • Degree of abnormality correlates with the
    severity of the dystonic symptoms
  • Animal studies suggest that repetitive
    input through training causes enlargement
    and fusion of representations
38
Q

final summary: somatosensory processing

A
  • Touch is processed in Somatosensory Cortex that maps the areas of the body disproportionally (larger processing areas for body sites with high sensitivity)
  • Body Map is plastic and adapts to sensory experience and learning
    (within limits)
  • Plasticity can be adaptive but also maladaptive (dystonia)
38
Q
A