L12 article on affect amplification and mental imagery Flashcards
1
Q
What is the article about?
A
This study investigated positive mood amplification, a characteristic feature of the bipolar disorder spectrum (BPDS). The researchers aimed to better understand the cognitive mechanisms underlying this elevated mood, specifically focusing on the role of mental imagery as a potential ‘emotional amplifier’
2
Q
What is mental imagery?
A
- the experience of perception in the absence of eliciting sensory input
- mental imagery of past, present, future, or fantasy events triggers affective processing in a manner like perception
- it is identified as a potential transdiagnostic risk mechanism and treatment target in a number of mental disorders
- Consequently, mental imagery paradigms are a potent experimental tool for manipulating affect (e.g., picture-word cue imagery generation paradigm)
- In BPDS, mental imagery is hypothesized to drive pathological mood amplification, exacerbating both manic and depressed states
3
Q
Participants and experimental groups
A
- The study compared two groups of young adults: those reporting high (n=31) versus low (n=30) hypomanic-like experiences based on their scores on the Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ)
- Participants were randomly assigned to generate either ‘elated’ or ‘calm’ positive mental imagery
- Their momentary affect was rated four times during the experimental session (before, twice during, and after the imagery task)
4
Q
What were the hypotheses?
A
- H1 Stimulus-specificity: Individuals in the high MDQ group would show a greater increase in affect when generating elated imagery compared to calm imagery, and compared to the low MDQ group
- H2 Affect-specificity: They predicted that mood amplification would be related to particular affective clusters. Specifically, they hypothesised:
- Increased positive (compared to negative) affect
- Increased active, goal-directed (compared to calm, consummatory) positive affect
- This part of the hypothesis investigated whether the amplification effect was specific to certain categories of affect, particularly those associated with (hypo)mania
5
Q
Methods
A
- A subclinical BPDS-relevant sample was recruited based on MDQ scores
- Participants underwent psychiatric screening using the Mini International Neuro-psychiatric Interview (MINI) to exclude those with current or past (hypo)manic, depressive, or psychotic episodes
6
Q
Procedure
A
- Participants completed a standardised imagery generation training procedure (definition of mental imagery was discussed and were trained in generating mental imagery)
- They engaged in a computerised picture-word cue imagery generation task with either ‘elated’ (reward-pursuit, achievement) or ‘calm’ (peace, contentment) stimuli. All participants saw the same pictures, but the accompanying word cue varied by condition. (e.g. a photo of the university library was paired with the phrase ‘achieving my best’ in the elated condition and ‘reading a book’ in the calm condition). They were asked to look at the picture, read the word or phrase, and then close their eyes and generate a mental image which combined both the picture and the word(s)
- Affect was measured using an expanded version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Expanded form (PANAS+), which included additional mood words relevant to hypomania and mood instability - reported 4 times
- Vividness of imagery was rated after each trial
7
Q
Findings
A
- For the high MDQ group, affect increased steeply and in a sustained manner over time in the ‘elated’ imagery condition, and more shallowly in the ‘calm’ condition. The low MDQ group did not show this sustained amplification effect
- Time-series analysis indicated that for the high MDQ group, affect increased additively over time in both conditions, with a faster increase in the elated condition. For the low MDQ group, elated imagery led to a decelerating pattern of mood increase, while calm imagery showed no significant affect change. (picture 1)
- Cluster analysis of affect words identified three subtypes: ‘negative’, ‘calm-positive’, and ‘active-positive’.
- In the high MDQ group, mood amplification in the ‘elated’ imagery condition was most pronounced for the active-positive affect subtype (e.g., elated, excited, energetic). The task did not impact the negative affect cluster in either group.
- In the low MDQ group, positive affect showed a modest decrease over time, more pronounced for the calm-positive cluster
8
Q
Conclusions and Implications
A
- The study provides experimental evidence for the Emotional Amplifier Theory in the context of hypomanic-like mood amplification
- It demonstrates that not all positive mental images are equal in their potential to amplify mood in individuals with high hypomanic-like experiences - Elated, approach-related imagery led to sustained mood amplification
- The findings suggest that positive, calm imagery might be used to modulate positive mood amplification in (hypo)manic states and potentially improve positive affect without the risk of escalation
- The study highlights the potential for targeted mental imagery interventions to dampen maladaptive mood amplification while preserving beneficial positive mood
9
Q
Future suggestions
A
- The researchers suggest that future work should extend these findings to clinical BPDS samples and explore the link between imagery, creativity, and approach-motivation
- They also noted that the ‘hypomanic-like’ affect words added to the PANAS+ require validation