Karen Gonsalkorale Flashcards

1
Q

James, 1890

A

Duality of the self
Self as an object that can be observed: “Me”
–Self-concept
Self as an agent doing the observing: “I”
–Self-awareness

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Brewer & Gardner, 1996

A

Personal (individual) self
–Beliefs about private self

Relational self
–Self in context of interpersonal relationships
E.g., intimate relationships

Collective self
–Self in relation to group memberships
Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Cultural Differences in Defining the Self

A

People from Western cultures tend to:

  • -Value independence and uniqueness
  • -Define themselves as quite separate from other people: Independent view of the self

The squeaky wheel gets the grease.
— American proverb

People from Asian and other non-Western cultures tend to:

  • -Value interdependence and connectedness
  • -Define themselves in terms of their relationships to others: Interdependent view of the self

The nail that stands out gets pounded down.
— Japanese proverb

May be different for bi-cultural people/ different selves activated at different times: In HK study looked at people fluent in 2 languages, given ‘I am’ test in each language. In English relational self stressed, in Chinese Interdependent selves.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Gender Differences in Defining the Interdependent Self

A

Women: more relational interdependence - focus more on their close relationships
E.g., how they feel about their spouse or their child

Men: more collective interdependence - focus on their memberships in larger groups
E.g., Australian, belong to the cricket team

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Nisbett and Wilson (1977)

A

How Do We Know Who We Are? Introspection

Looking inward – examining our thoughts and feelings

Problem: We don’t always have access to our internal states or their causes

Participants chose a pair of 12 stockings from a display
All the stockings were identical, when participants asked why nobody said the right-hand bias. They came up with causes for their actions like shiny/ resiliant. Noone said/ realised the stockings were all the same.
Showed a right-hand bias
We don’t know the reasons for our decisions

Introspection is not always reliable

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Wilson, Laser, and Stone (1982):

A

“Introspection” group (diary group):
In a diary, recorded
1) various factors that could influence mood eg. sleep deprivation, Mondays and
2) their mood
Estimated the extent to which various factors influenced their moods
“Observer” group:
Estimated the extent to which various factors influence mood
Result: The observer group was just as accurate as the introspection group. As the accuracy was the same. everyone is relying on general causal theories about what things could influence mood.Introspection doesn’t lead to insight about the causes of our moods

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Bem, 1967

A

How Do We Know Who We Are?Observing our Behaviour
Self-perception Theory
We infer who we are from what we do (our behaviour). Eg visit sick relative, I must be caring
Likely to occur when:
We are unsure of our attitudes and feelings
Our internal cues (attitudes and feelings) are weak
We have no clear situational influence on our behaviour
We chose the behaviour freely

If there’s an external cue eg. emergency bell we attribute behavior to that, it there’s no external cue we attribute behavior to internal

But how do we work out whether the situation is sufficient to explain our behaviour?
We ask ourselves: Is the behaviour freely chosen?
Intrinsic motivation: desire to engage in an activity because of internal reasons (you enjoy it)
Extrinsic motivation: desire to engage in an activity because of external rewards or pressures
If due to intrinsic motivation, then we will infer attitudes and feelings from behaviour

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Lepper, Green, Nisbett, 1973

A

Self-perception, Rewards, and Motivation

Rewards decrease intrinsic motivation

Got preschoolers to draw

1) First group told they would get given a reward for drawing
2) Given a reward afterwards, weren’t expecting it
3) No reward or mention of reward

2 weeks later measured how long children would draw with pencils. Children promised rewards showed less interest in drawing, spent 1/2 as much time drawing. Kids promised reward from outset don’t believe they’re drawing because they like it, but because of external reward. Rewards are more effective if they’re a surprise, or if you reward the effort or emphasise the element of choice

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Cooley, 1902

A

Looking Glass Self

We see ourselves as a reflection of how others see us

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Festinger, 1954

A

Social Comparison Theory

We learn about our own abilities and attitudes by comparing ourselves to other people
The theory revolves around two questions:
WHEN do you engage in social comparison?
With WHOM do you choose to compare yourself?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Gilbert, er al. 1995

A

Automaticity of Social Comparisons

Making social comparisons may be a spontaneous, automatic process, at least with salient targets

This means that we sometimes make social comparisons even when it is inappropriate to do so
but we can “undo” them if we have sufficient cognitive resources

Participants told that they would be doing a test of their “schizophrenia detection ability”

Watch an instructional video of a model doing the test. The model:
performs poorly (4/18 correct) OR well (16/18)
in both conditions, participants were told that the model’s performance was staged

Participants either made cognitively busy (memorise 8-digit number) or not

All participants do schizophrenia-detection task, receive ambiguous feedback (10/18)

Participants rate own competence at the task

When cognitively busy, context of model’s performance not considered. Thus, performance affects self-judgments
(sig diff between blue and yellow bars)

When not busy, context is taken into account and self-ratings are not significantly influenced by the model (no sig diff between blue and yellow bars)

May be evidence that social comparison is something we do all the time, at least with salient targets. So sometimes we make social comparisons in situations where it’s not appropriate

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Different Motives, Different Social Comparisons

A

If you want to know the top level to which you can aspire, you engage in upward social comparison:
comparing yourself to people who are better than you are on a particular ability

If you want to feel better about yourself, you engage in downward social comparison: comparing yourself to people who are worse than you on a particular trait or ability

Who you compare yourself to maps onto different self-motives

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Leary, 2007 What is a self-motive?

A

“An inclination that is aimed toward establishing or maintaining a particular state of self-awareness, self-representation, or self-evaluation”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Types of Self-motives

A

Accuracy perception (e.g., Trope, 1986): The motivation to have accurate and valid information about ourselves

Self-verification (e.g., Swann, 1987): The motivation to confirm what we already know about ourselves

Self-enhancement (e.g., Kunda, 1990): The motivation to maintain or increase the positivity of the self; the desire to maintain, protect, and enhance one’s self-image. eg. We are better than average, We are better today than we were in the past

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

College Board Survey, 1976-77

A

Self-enhancing Social Comparisons: The Better-Than-Average Effect

A study of a million high school students rated themselves as better than average at getting on with others.

We think we are above average on:
happiness (Wojcik & Ditto, 2014)
the ability to make objective judgments (Armour, 1999, cited in Pronin et al., 2004)

People exaggerate their skills and abilities in order to think well of themselves

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Ross & Wilson, 2000; Wilson & Ross, 2000

A

Past selves provide opportunity for downward comparisons

Participants were randomly assigned to rate self or acquaintance:
on positive and negative traits (e.g., socially skilled, self-confident vs. immature, narrow-minded)
now, and in past (approximately 3.5 months earlier)

Participants thought they were more more socially skilled an mature now then 3 months earlier, did not think so for acquaintances

Show derogation of past for self,
not acquaintance >self-enhancement

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

Lau & Russell, 1980

A

Biased Attributions

Newspaper quotes from winners and losers

Coded players’ and managers’ explanations for outcome

Results:
% making internal attributions:
winners: 80%
losers: 53%

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
18
Q

Dunning et al. 1991

A

The self-enhancement motive can influence how we define concepts

Defining categories: What does it mean to be a “good” son or a “good” daughter?
Whatever WE are: dependable, dutiful, obedient
vs. thoughtful, loving, caring

Defining traits: What does it mean to be “dependable”?
Always remembers to do things vs. makes a conscious effort to do things…

> We define categories and traits in self-serving ways

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
19
Q

Berglas and Jones (1967)

A

Bogus intelligence test (analogies test)
Analogies were solvable or unsolvable

Participants were all told they did well
Participants with solvable made internal attributions (I’m smart)
Participants with unsolvable analogies made external attribution for success (luck)

Given a second test

Can choose one of two experimental drugs
One improves intellectual functioning and performance
The other impairs performance

Who picked what?
“Solvable” participants picked enhancing drug
“Unsolvable” participants picked impairing drug
–>Built-in excuse for expected failure
–>Any success would make them look really smart

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
20
Q

Tesser & Campbell, 1982

A

Self-evaluation Maintenance Model

When someone outperforms us, we can respond by:
 basking in reflected glory (BIRGing)
My son is the best surgeon in the world
I went to school with Hugh Jackman
OR
 engaging in social comparison
-->we’ll look bad by comparison

Factors that Determine Responses to Being Outperformed

Closeness:
You can’t BIRG with a stranger
BUT: You’re more likely to compare yourself to people who are close

Self-relevance:
If the person is close and domain is relevant > social comparison
If the person is close and domain is not self-relevant > BIRG

Model says there are different things you can do: reduce closeness to people, reduce the relevance of the domain, work harder to reduce the gap or sabotage another’s performance

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
21
Q

Tesser & Smith, 1980

A

Sabotaging Another Person’s Performance

“Password” game
E.g., Password = Olympics
Easy clues: sports, medals
Difficult clues: Greek, pride

Manipulated relevance
Task described as measure of verbal skills OR unrelated to important skills

Each person in turn assumed the role of “identifier”, remaining 3 chose the clues (graded in difficulty)

One member of each pair: given bogus feedback that they had done badly as the “identifier”

DV: difficulty of clues chosen for friend vs. stranger

Depended on relevance:
Gave harder clues in relevant condition

This effect interacted with closeness
Under low relevance: helped friends more than strangers
Under high relevance: helped strangers more than friends > Gave friends HARDER clues than strangers!

Follow-up study replicated these findings with female participants, although the effects were weaker . May be because of how men and women define themselves. Men more likely to think of collective-self women relational-self so women aren’t as threatened when friends do well

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
22
Q

Steele, 1988; see Sherman & Cohen, 2006

A

Self-affirmation Theory

People are motivated to protect the integrity of the self

Motivations to protect self-integrity can result in defensive responses
Defensive responses diminish the threat and restore the integrity of the self (eg. sabotage, biased attributions, don’t have to do this)

The self-system is flexible
People can respond to threats in one domain by affirming the self in another domain

People can be affirmed by engaging in activities that remind them of “who they are”

Different domains of self: roles, (Student, parent) values (humor, religion), group identity (race, culture, religion) All domains through which we ensure we have effects of self-integrity

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
23
Q

Sherman & Kim, 2005

A

Self-affirmation and Biased Attributions

Field study with athletes (team sports) as participants

Team had either won or lost

Self-affirmed or not self-affirmed via classic self affirming method:

Manipulate self-affirmation:
Please rank order how important these values are to you personally.
business/economics art/music/theatre
social life/relationships science/pursuit of knowledge
religion/morality government/politics

Now please write about your most important value and why it is important and meaningful to you. (vs. least important value and why it might be meaningful to someone else)

Threaten the self (individual or collective self)

Measure defensive responses

Estimated how much their team had contributed to the outcome of the game. When not affirmed, teams made more references to group attributions, winners thought the win was more due to the team. When affirmed the difference disappeared.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
24
Q

Creswell et al., 2005

A

Self-affirmation and Evaluative Stress

Stress: The process by which events are appraised as threatening
May arise from perceived threats to the self
Can self-affirmation reduce evaluative stress?

Participants were self-affirmed or not

Completed the Trier Social Stress Task
Delivered speech to hostile audience and counted aloud backwards from 2083 in 13s in front of hostile audience

Salivary cortisol measured at baseline, and at 20 min, 30 min, and 45 min poststress onset

Stress hormone cortisol typically peaks at 20 minutes past stress onset. At baseline, no difference, good because manipulation did not occur. 20 minutes later people who weren’t affirmed showed a significant rise in cortisol. At 45 min the difference is still there, they’re still experiencing higher levels of cortisol.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
25
Q

Limits to Self-affirmation

A

Culture may moderate effects of self-affirmation:
Who finds what threatening?
Who finds what affirming?

Affirmation in domains that are related to the threat is less effective >Increases commitment to the identity at stake

There is a difference to how easterners & westerners see themselves. something threatening to a W may not be as threatening to an E, eg criticism of self vs criticism of family members

Affirmations in the moral domain can backfire
—After purchasing environmental products, people were more likely to cheat after proving their worth as moral people. Also, when people feel they don’t have to prove moral credentials they feel this licenses them to show prejudice to people of different backgrounds

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
26
Q

Kruger, 1999

A

College Board Survey (1976-77):
89% of nearly 1 million high school students rated themselves as above average on the ability to get along with others

Considered range of skills assessed in the College Board Survey, according to difficulty…

Ability to get along with others very easy, 89% rated themselves >m. Mechanics very difficult only 38% rated >m. We may not be engaging in social comparisons, basing ratings just on ourselves.

Comparative ability judgments are egocentric

We base our assessments on our own level of ability – How skilled am I?
If the task is easy, we think we’re good at it
If the task is hard, we think we’re not good at it

We don’t take into account the skills of the comparison group – we don’t ask: How skilled are my peers?

Compare self to other students in class on 8 skills:
4 easy (e.g., using a computer mouse)
4 difficult (e.g., computer programming)
  (percentile estimates)

Domain difficulty rated by a third party

For each skill, make absolute ratings of:
own ability, peers’ ability (i.e., the average student’s ability) (1=very unskilled, 10 = very skilled)

Absolute ratings of own ability predicted the comparative judgments (the percentile estimates).

However, absolute ratings of peers’ ability did not.

Thus, egocentric judgment accounts for the above average effect

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
27
Q

Heine & Hamamura, 2007

A

Cultural Differences in Self-Enhancement

Westerners were significantly more likely to self-enhance than East Asians
—-85/91 studies, of which 53 yielded “large” effect size, 28 “moderate”, 7 “small”

Within the Western samples, there was a significant self-enhancing bias (44/48)

Within the Eastern samples:
significant self-enhancement bias (19/46)
significant self-criticism bias (20/46)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
28
Q

Heine et al., 2001

A

Cultural Differences in Responses to Failure

Canadian students of European origin and Japanese students
Remote Associates Test
“sleep”, “fantasy”, “day”- relates to dream
Manipulated success vs. failure
Easy version of the test and scored well above the 50th percentile, or
Difficult version and scored well below the 50th percentile
Were given the opportunity to work on the task again “if they wanted”

When Westerners did well, they want to keep working and keep feeling good about themselves. If told you fail, they disengage. Japanese participants work on average 4 minutes longer then if told they did well on the task. May be because there’s more influence on self-improvement.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
29
Q

Cultural Differences in Self-enhancement

A

Both Westerners and Easterners self-enhance, but they self-enhance on different traits (Sedikides et al., 2003)
Westerners: individualistic attributes (“independent”, “self-reliant”, “unique”)
Easterners: collectivistic attributes (“cooperative”, “good listener”, “self-sacrificing”)
Easterners tend to engage in relationship enhancement (Endo et al., 2000)
See their family, friends, and romantic partners as better than themselves

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
30
Q

Taylor & Brown, 1998

A

Self-enhancing biases are examples of “positive illusions” that may promote mental health

  • Associated with happiness/contentment
  • Promote capacity for creative, productive work (e.g., via greater motivation, persistence)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
31
Q

von Hippel & Trivers, 2011

A

Self-enhancing biases are a form of self-deception that may help us to deceive others (evolutionary perspective)
We tell lies and deny failures a lot
-We frequently deceive others in order to acquire resources
-We will be more successful at deceiving others if we deceive ourselves (process information in a biased manner) self-enhancing may be a form of self-deception
-Self-enhancing biases boost our self-confidence, which enables us to advance socially and materially

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
32
Q

Epley and Whitchurch (2008, p. 1162)

A

Morphed attractive and unattractive faces. They were the actual participants who had been morphed to look more attractive or unattractive. Participants picked options that were 10% more attractive then they actually were. Evidence of people being self-deceptive.

Participants also faster to pick face that was 20% more attractive then there actual face when faces were flashed at them, and significantly slower to pick a face that was ugly. In our minds eye, we are more attractive then we actually are, this gives us more confidence and we are better at promoting ourselves as partners who are worthy of a mate.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
33
Q

Swann, 1987; 1990; 2012

A

Self-verification Theory

Don’t always want to self-enhance

Sometimes we want our self-views verified

  • -Even negative ones!
  • -We want other people to see us how we see ourselves

Self-verification: The motivation to confirm what we already know about ourselves

34
Q

Why do we self-verify and what strategies do we use?

A

Why Self-verify?
Serves to stabilise self-views

Stable self-views can be used to:

  • -Make predictions about the world
  • -Guide behaviour: We like opera, know Karmen is playing, know that we will like the show
  • -Maintain a sense of coherence

Origin of self-verification motive?

  • -Stable self-views promote group survival: need cooperation, know someone is good at building huts, they can build the huts
  • -Self-verifying information is more readily processed and fosters positive affect

Strategies for Self-verification

People satisfy the need for self-verification by constructing self-verifying “opportunity structures”

Strategies:

  • -Seek self-verifying partners
  • -Communicate self-views to others: talk about politics so people think you’re smart
  • -Display identity cues
  • -Enact behaviours that will bring others to see you as you see yourself
  • -“See” self-verifying evidence
  • -Biased attention and recall
35
Q

Swann and colleagues, e.g., 1981;1992; 2002

A

Evidence for Self-verification

Series of studies looking at strategies for self-verification (seeking self-verifying partners, communicating self-views, biased attention)

Recruited participants who had either a positive or negative view of themselves. People with negative self-views provide evidence, 70% of people have good self-vies, 30% have bad.

In one study:
Participants indicated whether they would prefer to interact with a positive evaluator or a negative evaluator
They were asked to “think aloud” into a tape recorder as they chose an evaluator

“Think aloud” responses indicated that participants preferred partners who made them feel they knew themselves, e.g.,
“Yeah, I think that’s pretty close to the way I am. [the negative evaluator] better reflects my own view of myself, from experience”

75% of people with negative self-view chose negative evaluator whereas only 18% of people with positive self-views did.

=>We seek self-verifying interaction partners – prefer to interact with partners who evaluate us the same as we see ourselves

36
Q

Swann & Read, 1981

A

Evidence for Self-verification ct’d

In another study, participants interacted with someone who they suspected viewed them differently to how they viewed themselves
These participants behaved in ways that confirmed their existing self-views—if they see you as nice, act mean; if they see you as mean, act nice
> We communicate our self-views to others

In another study: participants were told that another person had evaluated them in a positive or negative manner
Participants spent longer reading the passage if it was consistent with their self-view
> We pay more attention to self-verifying information

37
Q

Alternative Explanations for Self-Verification

A

Whole theory rests on people with negative self-evaluations

Flawed personalities
But participants with negative self-views express ambivalence about choosing the negative evaluator, said ‘I like the positive evaluation, but I’m not sure it sounds like me.’

Perceived similarity
If we think the same way, maybe we have things in common. Is possible but in the think aloud people didn’t spontaneously measure this

Winning converts
People may be trying to ‘win over’ the other, trying to convince the person they’re wrong. Some said this, but this was limited to positve people who were picking people with different views. People with negative self evaluations rarely did this at all

38
Q

Seih et al., 2013, Study 1

A

Cultural Differences in Self-verification

Indians and North Americans
Completed questionnaire on self-view specifically, self-perceived sociability

Imagined that two people had reviewed their answers on the questionnaire, and had given evaluation

Positive “I’d say this person probably feels comfortable and at ease around other people
Negative: “I get the feeling that this person doesn’t seem socially confident”
DV: Rated how accurate they thought the evaluation was

Participants with positive s-e thought the positive reviews were good, the negative reviews were bad. The reverse for participants with negative self views.

In Indian sample, evidence of self-verification, North American sample had the same effect. The difference was larger for NA, shows the effect was stronger

Not clear why there’s such a difference

39
Q

Kwang and Swann (2010)

A

Self-enhancement vs. Self-verification

meta-analysis

All studies had a measure of pre-existing self-views

SV stronger when measuring cognitive responses
SE stronger when measuring affective responses

Compared effect size for self-enhancement to effect size for self-verification as a function of various dimensions

When rejection risk is:
high (e.g., dating), self-enhancement > self-verification
low (e.g., married), then self-verification > self-enhancement

See slides (lecture 22) for more details

40
Q

Swann, Bosson, & Pelhman, 2002

Is self-verification strategic?

A

Strategic self-verification

People want their partner to view them positively on relationship-relevant dimensions (e.g., loving and caring)
But prefer to seek self-verifying evaluations on characteristics that are lower in relationship-relevance (e.g., artistic)

It’s not a case of which, but when…
determined by current needs and opportunities to self-enhance vs. self-verify

41
Q

Is Self-verification Adaptive?

A

Recall Taylor and Brown’s (1988) argument that self-enhancing biases are examples of “positive illusions” that promote psychological health

However, Swann (e.g., 2012) argues that non self-verifying information could be bad for your health. Specifically, people who have negative self-views:
show threat responses to positive feedback (Mendes &; Akinola, 2006)
are more likely to get sick after positive life events (Shimizu & Pelham, 2004)

Also recall von Hippel and Trivers’ (2011) argument that self-enhancing biases can help us to acquire resources

Swann (e.g., 2012) argues that self-verification should also promote survival

  • -people who self-verify are more predictable to other group members
  • -being on the same page with your relationship partner should facilitate activities such as successful child-rearing

When is Self-verification Maladaptive?

When people have developed inappropriate negative self-views, e.g.,

  • -thinks they are stupid, but they are smart
  • -thinks they are fat, but they are thin

In such cases, people’s self-views can make them behave in maladaptive ways

Thus, although self-verification can be adaptive, it can also have negative consequences

42
Q

Self-Esteem

A

Global self-esteem (or “trait self-esteem”): a person’s overall self-evaluation or sense of self-worth
the way people generally feel about themselves
typically measured using Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSE; 1965):
“I feel I’m a person of worth”
“On the whole, I am satisfied with myself”

Correlates of Global Self-esteem
(Relatively) high global self-esteem is correlated with a range of positive outcomes in several domains, including:
Academic achievement
Physical health
Psychological health

However, in some domains, the findings are less clear cut E.g., in the domain of interpersonal behavior, high global SE individuals think they are more popular, but they aren’t

43
Q

Methodological Issues of Self-Esteem

A

Shared method variance
Typically, both self-esteem and outcome variable measured using self-report
Correlations are lower when DVs are assessed using objective measures
E.g., self-reported vs. observer-rated physical attractiveness: r = .59 vs. r = .01 (Miller & Downey, 1999)

Correlation doesn’t imply causation
Need experimentation, longitudinal studies
3rd variable problem

44
Q

Trzesniewski et al., 2006

A

Longitudinal Data
Global self-esteem measured in early adolescence (age 11)

At age 26, assessed mental health, physical health, economic prospects, criminal behaviour

-Looked at self-reports and more objective 3rd party reports, as well as criminal records

Results: adolescent global self-esteem predicted better adjustment in early adulthood
Even when controlling for confounding variables (e.g., depression, SES)
Even for objective measures (e.g., criminal records)

45
Q

Baumeister et al. 1996

A

Self-esteem and Antisocial Behaviour

Antisocial behaviour, such as aggression, is not associated with low global self-esteem

Instead, antisocial behaviour is associated with a particular form of very high self-esteem

Ego-threat model
Individuals with this form of high self-esteem will aggress in response to self-threat
Applies to individuals who have over-inflated, unjustified views of themselves

Criminals did agree with narcissism inventories eg. If I ruled the world, it would be a much better place I am going to be a great person

46
Q

Brunell & Fisher, 2014

A

An aside: There is a debate over whether narcissists actually have low-self-esteem “deep down”, but there is at last some evidence suggesting that narcissists genuinely do think extremely well of themselves;

47
Q

Narcissism

A
  • “Passionately wanting to feel well of oneself” (Baumeister & Bushman, 1998, p. 10)
  • Overinflated sense of positive self-worth
  • Feel superior to other people
  • Self-centered and entitled
48
Q

Bushman & Baumeister, 1998

A

Self-esteem and Antisocial Behaviour: Testing the Ego-threat Model

Completed measures of narcissism and global self-esteem

Wrote an essay

Given a bad evaluation of essay (or not):
This is the worst essay I have ever read! vs.
No suggestions, great essay!

Given an opportunity to aggress against the person who had offended them (noise blasts)

Global self-esteem did not predict aggression

When threatened, narcissism was positively related to aggression

When praised, no relationship between narcissism and aggression

49
Q

Donnellan et al., 2005

A

Self-esteem and Antisocial Behaviour Outside the Lab

Adolescents and uni students

Study outside the lab suggests both narcissism and low self-esteem associate with antisocial behavior. Had self-reports and third party observers, was cross-sectional but could conduct analyses as if data sets. Different models looked at self-esteem and outcome variables and could make some convincing conclusions from the analysis.

Controlled for IQ, SES and supportive parenting, found narcissism positively related to antisocial behavior. More likely to engage in behavior like lying to parents, beating people up. Low SE also more likely to lie to parents, get into fights: both are problematic.

Global SE negatively related to outcome variables; higher SE= lower aggression.

Method:
Measured narcissism and global self-esteem
Outcome variables: Aggression, delinquent behaviour (self and informant reports)
Cross-sectional and longitudinal-type analyses
Controlled for SES, IQ, supportive parenting

Results:
Narcissism: Positively related to antisocial behaviour
Global self-esteem: Negatively related to antisocial behaviour

50
Q

Self-esteem and Antisocial Behaviour: Lab vs. Real-world

A

Lab: Low global self-esteem is NOT related to antisocial behaviour (Bushman & Baumeister, 1998)

Real-world: Low global self-esteem IS related to antisocial behaviour (Donnellan et al., 2005)

Why this discrepancy? The types of anti-social behaviours examined in the lab vs. real-world may differ in the degree to which they:

  • had obvious repercussions
  • are antisocial
  • noise blast is very artificial and it almost seems like the experimenter wants one to give the noise blast

However, both lab and real world studies find a link between narcissism and antisocial behaviour

Suggests that we need to distinguish between global self-esteem and an overinflated sense of self-worth

51
Q

Self-esteem Contingency

A

The extent to which self-esteem is contingent upon particular conditions being met (Deci & Ryan, 1995)

Domains of contingency may include acceptance, approval, family, self-reliance, morality, etc. (Crocker & Wolfe, 2001)

Individual differences in self-esteem contingency are associated with fluctuations in self-esteem

52
Q

Crocker & Wolfe (2001) Self-esteem Contingency

A

If SW is highly contingent on a particular domain, your SE is likely to go up and down. Study looked at uni students applying for lots of different programs. would get acceptances or rejections according to programs. People with low SE contingency for academics were the same when rejected or accepted, not so for those with high academic contingent self-esteem.

53
Q

Kernis et al., 2008 Self-esteem Stability

A

Interacts with levels of self-esteem
People with high, unstable self-esteem (“fragile” self-esteem) report that they are more prone to anger and hostility (Kernis et al., 1989) are more defensive (Kernis et al., 2008), more likely to interpret things as threats, much more defensive when asked to write essays describing when they’d done something bad.

Describe a time when you’ve broken the rules “I have honestly never done anything bad. Like the worst thing I do is burn CDs … I’ve honestly never drank anything. The only time I have drank anything was in Mexico and I was 18 at the time so that was legal … I’ve never like broken any rules.”

54
Q

Basic vs. Self-conscious Emotions

A

Basic emotions expressed by animals, universal across cultures. Also have distinct facial features and don’t require internal processes. Most non-human animals don’t have a sense of self-awareness, when they see a mirror reflection with red dot they don’t take the red dot off. Shows they don’t know who they are, suggests they lack self-awareness, suggests you don’t need self-awareness for emotions

e.g., anger, fear, sadness, joy

Self-conscious emotions
e.g., guilt, shame, embarrassment, pride

Less universal compared to other emotions, vary across cultures in terms of the extent to which emotions are valued, some emphasize shame and others don’t. What causes shame may also vary across cultures.

55
Q

(Tracy & Robins, 2007)

A

Features of Self-conscious Emotions

Require self-awareness and self-representations
which together enable self-evaluation

You need to have self-awareness; sense of agency ‘I’ and self-representation ‘me’. You need to know what other people are thinking of you, and that you’ve achieved a standard. Basic emotions may involve but they don’t have to, eg. fear

Are cognitively complex

Emerge later in life than do basic emotions
-Basics appear within first months of life, but the more cognitively complex don’t come until roughly the end of people’s third year when kids can have complex self-representations

Do not have universally recognised facial expressions- in order to recognise embarrassment you need to see the face and body, do have universal non-facial expressions, like body, way of moving

Facilitate attainment of social goals
Guilt may help you repair a relationship and give you access to greater resources. Shame may makes someone withdraw, stop doing something bad which allows one to survive indirectly, helps us to survive social rejection.

56
Q

Pride (Tracy & Robins)

A

Experienced when people believe they are responsible for a socially valued outcome or that they are a socially valued person, arises in response to internal attributions

Distinct non-verbal expression

-hands on hips and arms in air, expanded back posture deserving of status

Reliably recognised and distinguished from other emotions:
Adults in USA and Italy (Tracy & Robins, 2004)
Adults in Burkina Faso, West Africa (Tracy & Robins, 2008)- Least developed, live in mud huts with no western media, suggest universally recognized
Children as young as 4 years old (Tracy et al., 2005)

57
Q

Tracy & Matsumoto, 2008

A

2004 Olympic and Paralympic Games

Sighted, blind, and congenitally blind individuals from 30 nations displayed the pride expression in response to victory

They showed the same expressions: not only universal, also innate, something we come with.

58
Q

Functions of Pride

A

Expressing pride
Communicates success and social status to others
Pride expression is automatically associated with high status (Shariff & Tracy, 2009)

Experiencing pride
Feels good – reinforces behaviour
Motivate efforts to acquire skills that increase status and value to group
Pride is associated with perseverance on socially valued tasks (Williams & DeSteno, 2008)

59
Q

Williams & DeSteno, 2009

A

Pride as an Adaptive Emotion

Wanted to see how people who experience pride are evaluated by others: if adaptive should be seen as socially valued.

They were told they were to participate in a study to assist in the development of spatial tasks (to see if objects rotated are the same)

Individual task: measure of visuospatial ability

Pride manipulation:
Half of the participants received positive feedback on the individual task
Other half received no feedback (control condition)

Then participated in a group task (with one other participant and one confederate)

DVs:
Time spent manipulating the puzzle
Subjective ratings of participants’ dominance and likeability

Results: Proud participants
spent more time manipulating the puzzle
were perceived as more dominant
were liked better

Pride leads to functional outcomes, makes you more socially valued

60
Q

Tracy and Robins (2007, study 3)

A

“Think about a time when you felt very proud of yourself… describe the events that led up to your feeling this way in as much detail as you can remember”

“Rate how much you felt…”
like I was achieving, confident, fulfilled, productive, like I have self-worth, successful
> “Authentic pride”
arrogant, conceited, egotistical, pompous, smug, snobbish, stuck-up
> “Hubristic pride”

Authentic pride: “I am proud of what I did”
Achievement-oriented
Internal, unstable, controllable causes
“I won because I practised”
Positively correlated with global self-esteem

Hubristic pride: “I am proud of who I am”
Self-aggrandising 
Internal, stable, uncontrollable causes
“I won because I’m always great”
Positively correlated with narcissism
61
Q

Ashton-James & Tracy, 2012

A

“Everyone has, at some time in their life, felt a sense of accomplishment and self-worth. Think back to a time in your life where you felt like you had succeeded through hard work and effort, reached your potential, or achieved a goal.”

“Everyone has, at some time in their life, felt superior or better than other people. Think back to a time in your life where you behaved in a self-important manner, or felt pretentious or stuck-up.”

Were looking at prejudice and found hubristic pride associated with greater levels of prejudice compared to authentic.

62
Q

Cultural and Gender Differences in Pride

A

The value placed on pride differs across cultures
Pride is considered less desirable in collectivistic cultures (Eid & Diener, 2001)
Are the differences due to:
-the two facets of pride? (concept of pride may be linked more towards hubristic in Asian, authentic in Western)
-individual vs. collective pride?

Pride expression is better recognised when displayed by women than men (Tracy & Robins, 2008)
Men are assumed to experience more pride because of their higher status

63
Q

Goffman, 1963

dimensions of stigma

A

Stigma reduces an individual “from a whole and usual person to a tainted, discounted one”

Stigma Classifications

  1. Tribal stigma (e.g., ethnic groups)
  2. Abominations of the body (e.g., physical deformity)
  3. Blemishes of individual character (e.g., drug addict, homeless people, prostitutes supposedly reflect a poor moral character)
64
Q

Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998

content of stigma

A

Stigmatisation occurs when an individual possesses (or is believed to possess) “some attribute or characteristic that conveys a social identity that is devalued in a particular social context”

Believe it’s more useful to look at underlying content, not dimensions of stigma

Concealability (how visible):
-Readily visible stigma provides a schema that colours interpretations: hard like skin color or easy like sexual orientation/ religion? Important because if it’s visible, it provides a schema which colours interpretations of ones behaviours. If the stigma is not visible, you’re more likely to interact with someone on the basis of their characteristics.

-If unconceallable (visible), creates attributional ambiguity: if target is obvious it creates ambiguous attributions, targets don’t know if they’re treated the way they are because of skin colour or not. For eg. if you go to a job you don’t know if you don’t get it because of individual characteristics or stigma.

Controllability:

  • The extent to which a stigmatised person is seen as personally responsible for their stigma
  • Shape cognitive, emotional, and behavioural reactions
  • If not controllable people not seen as responsible for stigma. Judgements about contollability may vary widely, eg. sexual orientation. As an observer should I be blaming or helping this person? When we view people as responsible for their stigmas we treat them in a harsher way, eg. be meaner to homosexuals,
65
Q

What is the role of stereotypes relating to stigma?

A

In stigmatisation, “marks” become associated with negative evaluations and stereotypes (Jones et al., 1984)
–These evaluations and stereotypes are consensually shared

Negative evaluations and stereotypes becomes a basis for exclusion

66
Q

Abrams & Hogg, 1988

Functions of Stigma

A

Self-esteem and Social Identity

Denigrating others to feel good about yourself or your group

Engaging in downward social comparison (with groups that are worse off) helps to maintain a positive social identity (Abrams & Hogg, 1988)

-There is evidence of in-group bias: think more positive of our own group and treat members better. Is an incredibly robust finding

Doesn’t explain why certain groups are stigmatised within a society. Some groups are more likely to be stigmatised than others, doesn’t explain the consensual nature of stigmatisation

67
Q

Jost & Banaji, 1994

Functions of Stigma

A

Functions of Stigma: System Justification

People are motivated to justify existing social, economic, and political systems. In every society there is inequalities and we are motivated to justify the social hierarchy

Members of a society make attributions about deservingness, etc. based on existing social structure
E.g., Low SES people are lazy, stupid. This makes us think society is just and we feel good about this. Makes us think existing function is fair & legitimate

People low on social hierarchy tend not to have ingroup bias- don’t think their group is better. They maintain a sense of a just society by showing out-group, not in-group bias

Explains consensus within a culture about who is stigmatised, but doesn’t explain why some groups are stigmatised in almost every culture

68
Q

Kurzban & Leary, 2001

A

Functions of Stigma: Evolutionary Psychology Approach

Stigma does not simply arise from negative evaluations and stereotypes

Stigmatised individuals possess a characteristic that constitutes a basis for excluding them, characteristics that serve as a basis for exclusion are not arbitrary, for example “Australians are uncultured’

Characteristics are not arbitrary; There is commonality across cultures in what characteristics lead to stigmatisation. It’s adaptive to exclude certain people.
This understanding is often criticized, said it suggests we ought to be stigmatizing people, but no; it’s a way of understanding why.

There are universal characteristics associated with stigmatisation, e.g.
Poor partner for social exchange (“blemishes of individual character”, Goffman, 1963)
poor prospects (e.g., poor, infirm elderly, poor social connections) cheaters (e.g., criminals, convicts, con artists)
Carry parasitic infection (“abominations of the body”)
physically ill

69
Q

Lawrence Summers

A

there are group differences in ability

women are underrepresented in science and engineering because of a “different availability of aptitude at the high end”

70
Q

Steele (1999)

A

“From an observer’s standpoint, the situations of a boy and a girl in a math classroom or of a Black student and a White student in any classroom are essentially the same. The teacher is the same; the textbooks are the same; and in better classrooms, these students are treated the same. Is it possible, then, that they could still experience the classroom differently, so differently in fact as to significantly affect their performance and achievement there?”

-Explaining why, when controlling for minority group ability, there are still differences in outcomes/ performance

Stereotype Threat Theory

  • Success requires identification – leads to sustained achievement motivation
  • Identification requires perception of “good prospects”
  • If identification doesn’t form or is undermined, performance will suffer
71
Q

Barriers to Identification According to Stereotype Threat

A

Socio-structural variables: lack of resources to perform, discrimination, socio-economic disadvantage: don’t have access to resources because of background. There’s a lack of role models for minorities which makes it more difficult to achieve in a domain.

You need skills and opportunities to prosper, you also need the feeling that you belong and will be valued in that domain.

Stereotype threat

  • Know that a stereotype exists about your group
  • Worry that others will apply the stereotype to you
72
Q

Effects of Stereotype Threat?

A

Produces worry and anxiety: this uses up valuable cognitive resources which may be better spent elsewhere, results in

Impaired performance

Disidentification
Even if you manage to perform well, it is not fun to work in a field where you assume everyone thinks you’re no good. Women in phsyics after school/ uni / work may decide there’s too many barriers

73
Q

Features of Stereotype Threat

A

Not tied to any one group
White males feel stereotype threat when performing a math test with Asian participants.
-White males may also feel the burden of being racist and this makes them more racist when performing on implicit measures tests.

Driven by situational relevance of stereotypes
There is something about the situation that makes the stereotype salient you may be reminded explicitly of stereotype or girl may just be surrounded by boys in a classroom

Doesn’t require internalisation: don’t have to believe the stereotype is true in order to be affected by it.

Overcoming stereotype threat requires new proof with every new challenge
Self-doubt can then emerge when task is very difficult

74
Q

Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1997

A

Women and Maths Performance
Matched women and men on maths skill

Took a difficult maths test

Told that the test had been shown to:
-produce gender differences
or
-not produce gender differences

-When told the test was known to produce gender differences, there was a huge difference in performance; girls under performed, when not told (the stereotype was not made salient) small difference in performance between genders. Subtle variations of a situation can change results. Stereotypes is one of these contextual factors

75
Q

(Steele & Aronson, 1995)

A

African Americans and Test Performance

Black and White Stanford students took a verbal test

Test framed as diagnostic or not diagnostic of verbal abilities
“test of your verbal abilities”
or
“a problem solving task”

Test of verbal skills activated expectation that AA would be worse. When test was framed as diagnostic of verbal abilities, AA under-performed. Shows evidence of stereotype threat.

A later part of the study got people to complete letters (could be completed with ‘Race’ or ‘lace’ etc.) To see the extent stereotypes were activated. Greater stereotype activation= lower performance.

Their 4th study found that just asking participants to complete their race before completing a test lead to a decrement in performance.

76
Q

Yeung & von Hippel, 2008

A

Wanted to look at the stereotype women are bad drivers

88 females, all held drivers licence

Experimental conditions:
Stereotype threat: Purpose of study was to study why men are better drivers than women
Control: Mental processes involved in driving

Task: Driving simulator

DV: hitting jaywalker

60% of women in the stereotype threat condition hit the jaywalker compared to 20% in the control. Performance increments may occur in many domains, even hitting a jaywalker.

77
Q

Johns, Schmader, & Martens, 2005

A

Reducing Stereotype Threat

Men and women completed a maths test

Prior to the test, they were randomly assigned to 3 conditions:
Problem-solving condition
Maths-test condition
Teaching-intervention condition:
“it’s important to keep in mind that if you are feeling anxious while taking this test, this anxiety could be the result of these negative stereotypes that are widely known in society and have nothing to do with your actual ability to do well on the test”

Women were affected by math test condition, not problem solving or teaching condition.

78
Q

Dar Nimrod & Heine, 2006

A

Examined the effects of genetic vs. experiential accounts of gender differences in performance

Participants overheard the fake debriefing of “a participant in a different study”

“The study that you participated in was designed to add to the recent findings made by an international team of researchers led by scientists from Yale and Oxford who have found that…”

Stereotype Threat Condition (S)
“… females are constantly aware of their gender, even if only at a subconscious level…”
No Gender Difference Condition (ND)
“… contrary to some popular beliefs, there are no gender differences in math ability…”
Genetic Condition (G)
“… gender differences in mathematical ability are caused by genetics…”
Experiential Condition (E)
“… gender differences in mathematical ability are caused by early childhood experiences…”- women think their may be differences in ability, but this doesn’t apply to me…

Women in the experiential and no gender difference conditions performed the best on math tests

79
Q

When Do We Engage in Social Comparison?

A

When there’s no objective standard to measure yourself against
e.g., How much money should you donate to charity?

When you experience uncertainty about yourself in a particular area
e.g., You scored 15/20 on a test. Is this a good score?

80
Q

With WHOM Do We Choose To Compare Ourselves?

A

We compare ourselves to people who are similar to ourselves

Because we are motivated to perceive ourselves accurately

Festinger (1954) Social Comparison Theory

81
Q

What is the role of social context relating to stigma?

A

Stigma is a social construct which a society creates, stigma in the eye of the beholder, not the person.

Society determines who is stigmatised and who is not (Crocker et al., 1998)
Variability across time and cultures

Irish catholics used to be considered worthy of exclusion: variability across time. Also across cultures: being overweight may be considered bad here but not in other cultures.