V1 Flashcards

1
Q

Hubel and Wiesel’s discovery of receptive fields in the striate cortex (V1):

A

Recorded neural activity in the primary visual cortex (V1) of cats

Used light stimuli presented on a screen while measuring neuron firing

Key Findings:
Unlike retinal or LGN neurons, V1 neurons were not excited by simple spots of light

Instead, they responded best to edges, bars, and lines with:
-Specific orientations
-Specific positions in the visual field
-Movement in a preferred direction (for some)

✅ Conclusion:
V1 neurons are tuned to orientation, not just light intensity — they act as edge detectors.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Binocularity and ocular dominance

A

In primary visual cortex (V1), inputs from the two eyes are initially segregated in layer 4C, where:
Magnocellular input (motion, depth) → 4C𝛼
Parvocellular input (color, detail) → 4C𝛽
Each LGN layer projects to V1 with monocular (one-eye) input only
Above and below layer 4C, neurons begin to receive input from both eyes — these are called binocular neurons

Ocular Dominance Columns
In layers outside 4C, neurons are organized into ocular dominance columns:
-Alternating bands of neurons in V1 that prefer left or right eye input
-Each column is more strongly driven by one eye, but many neurons are binocular

Role of binocular neurons?
* Perfectly suited to compare the retinal images of the 2 eyes (depth perception/stereoscopic vision), which show slightly different views of the word –> Helps perceive relative depths of objects in the environment
* Binocularity also increases the sensitivity of the visual system

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Orientation selectivity

A

V1 neurons (especially simple cells) respond best to bars or edges of light at a specific orientation (e.g., vertical, horizontal, diagonal).

V1 neurons are also tuned to:
-Spatial frequency (how wide or narrow the stripes are)
-Gratings (patterns of repeated bars)
-Rectangular ON/OFF regions (not circular like retinal/LGN neurons)

How are these orientation-selective receptive fields formed?
👨‍🔬 Hubel & Wiesel’s Model:
Multiple LGN neurons, each with circular receptive fields, converge onto a single V1 simple cell.
-These LGN inputs are arranged in a line — their overlapping ON regions form a rectangular excitatory zone, surrounded by inhibition.
-🧩 The result:
The V1 neuron now responds best when a bar of light aligns with the axis of this arrangement.

If the bar is tilted or misaligned, fewer LGN inputs are activated → weaker response

✅ This creates a selective response to orientation.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Directional Motion Sensitivity

A

Some V1 neurons are direction-selective: they respond strongly to motion in one direction, and weakly or not at all in the opposite or orthogonal directions.
-This helps the brain detect object movement and motion flow.

Directional Tuning Curve (Polar Plot)
Plots firing rate (Hz) as a function of movement direction (360° around a circle).
-The farther from the center, the higher the firing rate.
- A neuron with directional preference will have a peak in the direction it prefers.

Tuning Curve Description:
On a polar plot:
180° (downward) = longest line (highest firing rate)
0° or 360° (upward) = shorter line (lower response)
90° and 270° (horizontal) = no line (no response)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Colour contrast detection

A
  • Cells in the parvocellular pathway provide information about red-green light (lower 4C) (4C𝛽)
  • Cells in the koniocellular pathway provide information about blue-yellow light (upper 4C layers in blobs) - Directly to blobs between layers (Blobs are color-sensitive regions within layers 2/3 of V1)

Ultimately, these pathways lead to blobs (have solar sensitive cells) in V1
Layers 3,4,5,6 -> p-ganglion, position/colour sensitive (green, red)
Layers in between m & p -> koniocellular

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Simple cells

A

cortical neurons whose receptive fields have clearly defined inhibitory and excitatory regions

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Complex cells

A

cortical neurons whose receptive fields do not have clearly defined inhibitory and excitatory regions
* E.g., may respond to a stripe presented anywhere in its receptive field (i.e., it is “phase-insensitive”

ex: A complex cell may respond strongly to a vertical bar, whether it’s left, center, or right within its receptive field.

In contrast, a simple cell would only respond if the bar is in just the right position (phase-sensitive).

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Ocular dominance columns

A

Neurons are organized into vertical columns that prefer input from either the left or right eye.

These columns are arranged in an interdigitated pattern (L–R–L–R…).

Each column spans all layers except layer 4C, where inputs are still segregated.

Distance between adjacent ODCs: ~0.5 mm

neurons are arranged in an interdigitated “left-preferring” and “right-preferring” manner

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Orientation Columns

A

Neurons within a column prefer the same orientation of stimulus (e.g., vertical, 45°, horizontal).

Orientation preference changes gradually across the cortex — forming a map of all possible angles.

These columns are organized in a radial (pinwheel) pattern, with the center of the pinwheel being a key landmark.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

old Hubel and Wiesel’s Hypercolumn Model (“Ice Cube Model”)

A

A hypercolumn:
A ~1 mm² block of V1 that contains:

One full set of orientation columns for each eye

So, 2 ocular dominance columns, each with all orientation preferences

It processes all visual features (form, motion, depth, colour) for a small region of the visual field

Each contains info about a very small part of the retina
* 1 mm block of cortex (each “sees” a different portion of the visual field)
* Distance between centre of adjacent ODCs is ~0.5 mm

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Colour Sensitivity and Blobs

A

🔬 Identified using cytochrome oxidase (CO) staining:
Blobs: clusters of CO-positive cells in layers 2/3 → high metabolic activity

Located in the center of ocular dominance columns (~0.5 mm apart)

Function: likely involved in colour processing

Receive parvocellular and koniocellular input → process fine detail and colour

➖ Interblob regions:
Lie between blobs

Process form and orientation (not colour)

Staining the visual cortex for cytochrome oxidase (CO), an enzyme that labels metabolically-active cells, reveals vertical columns in layer 2/3 called blobs (bands of CO positive cells)
Occur in centre of ocular dominance columns (~0.5 mm apart)
Function of blobs may be to process colour information
Blob and interblob regions are anatomically distinct
Linked to visual processing through 2 separate visual systems: magno and parvo (preferentially to blobs, cones: detail/colour)

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

updated hypercolumn model

A

Researchers use intrinsic signal imaging to measure brain activation:
Shine constant light on cortex → measure reflected light
Active regions absorb more light (due to deoxygenated hemoglobin), appear darker

Orientation columns are arranged in a pinwheel structure

Colour blobs are located at the centers of these pinwheels

Reinforces idea that orientation and colour are processed in parallel but spatially organized systems

Hypercolumn: a 1 mm block of striate cortex contains “all the machinery necessary to look after everything the visual cortex is responsible for, in a certain small part of the visual world” (Hubel, 1982)
2 ocular dominance columns, ODCs (L, R)
Each ODC covers every possible orientation
The centre of the orientation pinwheel contains a colour-sensitive “blob”

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Adaptation and tilt aftereffect

A

A reduction in neural response after prolonged or repeated exposure to a stimulus. It’s a powerful tool to probe how neurons are tuned to specific stimulus features like orientation.

Before Adaptation (Left Panel)
Each bar = a group of V1 neurons tuned to a specific line orientation (e.g., –10°, 0°, 10°, etc.).
When a vertical line (0°) is shown, neurons tuned to 0° respond most strongly, with nearby orientation-tuned neurons responding less.

After Adaptation (Right Panel)
The subject is exposed to 20° stripes for a prolonged period. Neurons tuned to 20° become fatigued, reducing their responsiveness.
-10° and 0° neurons are also slightly fatigued.
When a vertical (0°) bar is shown again: The –10° neurons (not fatigued) fire relatively more.
This shifts the population response toward the left.

Tilt AfterAffect: The brain interprets the shifted activity pattern as if the stimulus is tilted slightly leftward → this is a tilt aftereffect.
You perceive the vertical line as being tilted left (even though it’s not), because of the adaptation-induced imbalance in neural responses

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

The tilt aftereffect

A
  • Perceptual illusion of tilt, produced by adaptation to a pattern at a given orientation
  • Supports the idea that human visual system also has individual neurons selective for different orientations
  • after adaptation to tilted line stimulus, straight lines should appear to point left (if you do this with one eye closed, the tilt effect still occurs
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Ganglion Cells and Stripes

A

Retinal ganglion cells respond best to gratings (striped patterns) with a specific spatial frequency that matches the size of their receptive field.

Low spatial frequency (broad stripes):
Light and dark areas fall across both center and surround → cancels out → weak response

Medium spatial frequency (just right):
Aligns with center-surround structure → strongest response

High spatial frequency (fine stripes):
Too small → averages out over receptive field → weak response

Each cell acts like a “Goldilocks filter” → not too big, not too small, just right

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Adaptation to Spatial Frequency experiment

A

🔬 Procedure:
1.Observe a grating of a specific spatial frequency (e.g., 7 cycles/degree)

2.After 20 seconds, look at a neutral stimulus

  1. Your contrast sensitivity function (CSF) shifts — you temporarily lose sensitivity at that frequency

What This Shows:
The visual system has neurons tuned to different spatial frequencies
-Adaptation reduces the responsiveness of those neurons → leading to a dip in contrast sensitivity at that frequency
After adapting to 7 cpd: Your brain is less sensitive to patterns at 7 cpd
But still sensitive to higher or lower frequencies

Recordings from monkey striate cortex reveal neurons with different spatial-frequency tuning functions – all neurons work together

17
Q

Where are the neurons that encode adaptation?

A

Interocular Transfer Experiment:
You adapt to a stimulus (e.g., a grating) using only one eye (e.g., left eye). Then you test for the adaptation effect using the other eye (e.g., right eye).

What Happens?
Adaptation still occurs! Even though the test eye never saw the adapting stimulus, there’s a reduction in sensitivity (e.g., tilt aftereffect or spatial frequency dip is still perceived).

What Does This Mean?
Adaptation must happen in neurons that receive input from both eyes → i.e., binocular neurons
These are found in the primary visual cortex (V1) and beyond, not earlier in the visual pathway. Adaptation occurs in V1 or later in the visual pathway, where inputs from both eyes converge onto binocular neurons.

18
Q

interocular transfer

A

Transfer of adaptation from adapted to non adapted eye is called interocular transfer

  • Because inputs from the two eyes don’t converge until V1, adaptation must happen in cortical neurons (V1 or beyond)!
19
Q

Spatial Frequency–Tuned Pattern Analyzers

A

What Are They?
The visual system breaks down images using spatial frequency channels. Each channel is a group of cortical neurons tuned to respond best to a specific range of spatial frequencies. This concept is like a neural filter bank — different neurons are sensitive to different levels of visual detail.

Types of Frequency Channels:
1. Low spatial frequency: Broad outlines, general shapes, large-scale contrast
2. High spatial frequency: Fine detail, edges, sharp boundaries

Spatial frequency channel is a pattern analyzer, implemented by an ensemble of cortical neurons, in which each set of neurons is tuned to a limited range of spatial
frequencies

20
Q

How we study vision in infants
Preferential looking

A
  • Present infants with 2 stimuli
  • Measure time spent staring at each stimulus - can the baby still see w/decreased contrast?
  • Babies will preferentially look at grading
  • Problems with this method? Baby will get distracted, not cooperate based on other factors, how hungry/tired they are
21
Q

How we study vision in infants
Measure visually-evoked potentials (VEPs)

A
  • Attach electrodes to infant’s scalp and measure changes in electrical activity in the underlying cortex
  • Present infants with 2 stimuli
  • Measure electrical signals from the brain, very quick.
  • start with thick bars (low frequency) and then decrease thickness (higher frequency)
22
Q

baby Visual development

A

The rod system is functional in early infancy

Cone system: Infants < 1 month can’t discriminate colours; this ability emerges by 2-3 months

Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity for high spatial frequencies develop slowly and may not reach adult levels until several years of age… babies are not good at detecting detail/contrast (can see difference between black and white)

23
Q

Development of the CSF - Contrast Sensitivity Function

A
  • Sensitivity to low spatial frequencies develops sooner
  • Postnatal changes in the retina limit the development of acuity and contrast sensitivity
  • Foveal receptor density increases after birth → finer cone sampling in later childhood (cones develop after birth, density increases and shape, size and packing changes) happens after birth to contribute/inc. visual acuity
  • Migration of RGCs and inner nuclear layers away from the foveal region (foveal pit) → fovea isn’t fully adultlike until 4 years of age!

20’s we are most sensitive, loose contrast sensitivity at high frequency as we grow

24
Q

Consequences of abnormal visual experience during development

A

Cataract: opacity of the lens (light reaching both eyes is not equal, one becomes more blurry)

Strabismus: one eye is turned so that the two eyes see different views of the world (eyes are not aligned/ not working together)

Anisometropia: two eyes have very different refractive errors (one eye has different refractive error than the other. One can be very near sighted and the other is normal which causes a big disbalance and impaired development)

25
Critical period and Amblyopia
Critical period: a phase in the life span during which abnormal early visual experience can alter normal neural development * In humans it is the first 3-8 years (for vision) * The critical period is a time of neural plasticity. Cortical neurons are still wired to their presynaptic inputs (need this to develop adult vision, shape neural circuits) * Abnormal early visual experience can lead to misplaced neural connections, and can result in amblyopia (reduced visual acuity and lack of binocular depth perception - poor stereoscopic depth perception, weak eye and visual acuity)
26
Parallel visual channels: retina and beyond Magnocellular channel
(where they are/how they move) * Involved in processing information primarily from peripheral retina * Information is sent to layer 4C𝛼 in V1 * Greater sensitivity to motion * Sensitivity in M-system is fast
27
Parallel visual channels: retina and beyond Parvocellular channel
(slower/what object is - 3,4,5,6 LGN levels) * Involved in processing information primarily from foveal region (recall small RF size of midget RGCs) * Information is sent to layer 4C𝛽 in V1 * Greater sensitivity to colour and spatial aspects of a stimulus (e.g., object form, colour, detail) * Parvocellular neurons have higher spatial resolution
28
Parallel processing through extrastriate areas
Extrastriate cortex: a set of visual areas that lie just outside V1 Dorsal (where) pathway: locating objects in space and acting on them. Parietal Ventral (what) pathway: recognizing objects. Temporal.
29
Two Major Visual Pathways
1. Ventral "What" pathway Object recognition (form, color, identity) Parvocellular (P) pathway 2. Dorsal "Where/How" pathway Spatial awareness & motion (location, movement, action guidance) Magnocellular (M) pathway At early stages (LGN → V1), M and P channels are distinct. Physical segregation of 2 streams becomes more blurred at higher levels * Streams are preferentially influenced by M- or P- channels * Blurring is partly due to bidirectional information flow * Integration among different types of information allows for the formation of unified (we can see its movement + colour, features - what it is) percepts
30
The What Pathway: Inferotemporal (IT) Cortex
Located in the lower temporal lobe Receives highly processed input from earlier visual areas Crucial for identifying objects, faces, and meaningful stimuli Evidence: Monkey studies: Lesions to IT cortex → object recognition deficits -Human stroke victims: Damage to similar areas → visual agnosia (can't recognize objects despite intact vision)
31
Properties of IT Cortex Neurons
Large, central receptive fields Insensitive to simple features (lines, bars) Selective for complex, meaningful stimuli: Tools, places, people E.g., face-sensitive neurons
32
Face Processing in Humans 🔎 Homologous Area: Fusiform Face Area (FFA)
Identified via fMRI Responds strongly when faces are perceived Less active when viewing non-face objects (e.g., vase in face-vase illusion)
33
Face Processing in Humans Quiroga et al. (2005): The “Jennifer Aniston Cell”
Recorded from neurons in human temporal lobe Some neurons responded selectively to specific individuals Suggested idea of sparse coding — unique neurons for meaningful, familiar stimuli Some cells responded to very specific stimuli - specific faces - not any random face (e.g., Jennifer Aniston) Different cells respond to different features
34
Feedforward vs. Feedback Processing
Feedforward: Information flows one way: retina → V1 → higher areas Fast, general categorization (e.g., “That’s an animal”) No detailed analysis or awareness yet text: carry out a computation one neural step after another, without need for feedback from later stages (one cell -> next cell -> no need for feedback) * Can give crude information about objects (categories) and scenes (different brain response is you will categorize an object as an animal or not (category) - no need for feedback) * Complex acts of recognition require feedback connections, i.e., “re-entrant” information flows back down from higher level to lower-level visual areas 🔁 Feedback / Re-entrant Processing: Higher areas send information back to earlier ones Helps resolve ambiguity, fill in missing details Crucial for detailed recognition and awareness
35
Reverse Hierarchy Theory:
First, you get a quick, high-level impression via feedforward Then, you “zoom in” via feedback to clarify fine details → E.g., you know it’s a dog, then realize it’s your dog feedforward processes give you a general, categorical impression of the world but you don’t become aware of the details until “reentrant” feedback goes back down the visual pathway (feedback gives detailed info/category for identification, to determine more specific/what animal it is)