Evolutionary Arms Races Flashcards

1
Q

Types of prey defenses

A
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

Predator adaptations and prey counter-adaptations: searching for prey

A

Predator adaptations:
- develop a ‘search image’
- Improved sensory capacity
- Focus search on small area

Prey counter-adaptations:
- space out behaviorally
- crypsis
- polymorphic appearance

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

Predator adaptations and prey counter-adaptations: recognizing prey

A

Predator adapations:
- learning

Prey coubnter-adaptaions:
- warning signs of toxicity
- masquerade
- deception by mimicry

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

Predator adaptations and prey counter-adaptations: catching prey

A

Predator adaptations:
- secretive approach (motor skills, speed, agility)
- offensive weapons

Prey counter-adaptations:
- startle response (e.g. eyespots)
- signal to predator that it’s been detected
- escape flight

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

Predator adaptations and prey counter-adaptations: handling/eating prey

A

Predator adaptations:
- ‘subduing ability’

Prey counter-adaptations:
- toxicity
- e.g. spikes

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

Red queen evolution

A

If predators adapt and prey counter-adapt, does anyone ever really have the advantage?

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

Crypsis in moths

A
  • Cryptic-looking forewings (for camouflage?) but brightly-colored hindwings
  • Gave a slideshow to bluejays in an aviary
  • Backgrounds were cryptic or conspicuous
  • In some cases, a moth was present in the picture, in others not
  • If a moth was present, birds could get a mealworm as a reward for pecking at the slide
  • If no moth was present, the jay would peck a key to advance to another slide
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

Search images

A
  • Luc Tinbergen: birds would not eat certain insects each year when they appeared in the spring, but started to include them in their diet with tyme
  • Hypothesis: change due to an improvement over time in the birds’ ability to see the cryptic insects, aka ‘adopting a specific search image’

-Dawkins’ experiments gave support for this, using chicks and colored rice grains
- Orange grains on green or orange background
- Chicks found prey quicker on a conspicuous background (orange on green)
- On cryptic backgrounds (orange on orange), birds pecked at stones but after 3-4 minutes, located the cryptic grains and started to eat them
- By the end of the trial, ate both grain colors at equal rates on each background
- Supports the hypothesis that birds developed a ‘search image’ over time for cryptic prey

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

Search images and polymorphic prey

A
  • Some prey are what we call ‘polymorphic’ – different color morphs coexist in the same population
  • One hypothesis for polymorphism is that it disrupts search images – if a predator develops a search image for one morph, it may miss other morphs
  • Pietrewicz and Kamil tested this with their bluejay setup: bluejays got better and better at locating Morphs A and B when presented alone, but did not improve performance when morphs were mixed
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

Examples of camouflage beyond background-matching

A
  • Disruptive coloration
  • Countershading
  • Masquerade
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

Disruptive coloration

A
  • Bold, contrasting patterns on the periphery of the body, which serve to break up the body outline
  • Experiments with fake moths show that cryptic prey survive better than non-cryptic prey
  • But disruptive markings enhance survival further
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

Countershading

A
  • Darkening the dorsal (top) surface
  • Lightening the ventral (bottom) surface
  • Especially effective in marine or arboreal environments
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

Masquerade

A
  • Resemblance of inedible objects
  • Twigs, leaves, bird droppings, flowers, stones
  • Camouflage without crypsis: organism may be detected but is not detected as prey!
  • This is difficult to test experimentally – need to demonstrate that the animal was detected but not recognized
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

Testing masquerade

A
  • Skelhorn et al. used domestic chicks (predator) with twig-resembling caterpillars (prey) placed in clear view)
  • Birds in two treatment groups:
  • 1) Encountered natural twigs
  • 2) No experience with twigs
  • Birds with prior experience of natural twigs took longer to attack the caterpillars
  • Shows that caterpillars were detected, but misidentified = masquerade
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

Startle: dazzle and eyespots

A
  • Coloration that creates a startling effect in the predator
  • Startles or confuses the predator long enough that the prey can make a getaway
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

Warning coloration: aposematism

A
  • Some prey are really brightly colored, instead of cryptic
  • Why? Doesn’t this make them really easy for predators to spot?
  • Certain bright colors (red, yellow, or orange, often combined with black) are associated with defenses such as toxins, spines, or stings
17
Q

Do prey with defenses really benefit from being conspicuous?

A

-The desert locus exists in two morphs:
- When populations are low –> cryptic coloration
- Behaviorally, avoid one another, avoid feeding on plants with defensive chemicals

-When population densities are high –> crypsis no longer possible due to not enough habitat for background-matching –> adopt yellow and black coloration
- Behaviorally, group together (gregarious) and start to eat toxic plants to sequester toxic compounds

Sword et al:
-Fed lizards a distasteful, gregarious phase locust
-The next day, fed them another locust from a given treatment group
-Looked at how many lizards rejected the prey given prior experience with aposematic, toxic grasshoppers
- After one trial, predators learned to avoid unpalatable locusts when they are in their black and yellow color morph, rather than cryptic morph
- The change to aposematic is adaptive and reduces predation

18
Q

The evolution of warning coloration

A

-Which came first: distastefulness or bright colors?
-Either way poses a paradox:
- Say a mutation arises that makes a prey animal brightly colored
- Suddenly the prey is more obvious to predators –> more likely to perish
- If it was distasteful first: the predator may decide not to attack that kind of prey again, but if the mutation was rare, the mutation goes extinct and the predator never encounters it again
- If it was colorful first: the predator doesn’t learn anything to associate the colors with distastefulness, and may attack others with that mutation
- Thus, even though there is an advantage with predators, the mutation never has a chance to spread because the bright colors make the prey obvious
- Ronald Fisher proposes a solution

19
Q

Fisher’s solution to questions about the evolution of warning coloration

A

-Fisher noticed that many aposematic species lived in groups, whereas cryptic species do not
-Proposed that warning coloration can evolve if it arises in family group:
- Siblings are warning colored
- Even if the sampled individual perishes, the siblings are likely to benefit from reduced predation and the gene can spread
- Group selection
-Mathematical models support this, but reality challenges its underlying assumptions

20
Q

Challenges to Fisher’s assumptions

A

1) The ‘sampled’ (or attacked) individual always perishes
- False!
- Many bright-colored animals have additional defenses (e.g. hairs) that mean they don’t necessarily die from a predator attack
- Thus, there may be an advantage to the individual of warning coloration if it prevents subsequent attacks by a predator

2) Family grouping sets the stage for the evolution of warning colors
- In some groups (e.g. butterflies), warning coloration evolved before group living
- Thus, there may be an advantage to the individual of grouping because ti means the likelihood of an attack on any given individual is less likely

21
Q

Explain why mimicry of something distasteful is beneficial

A
  • If one species evolves warning coloration, it benefits from reduced predation
  • Selection could then favor the other species to mimic the first species, to also benefit from reduced predation
  • The association between bright colors and repellent defenses has led to the evolution of two forms of mimicry
22
Q

Müllerian mimicry: repellent species look alike

A
  • Fritz Müller: noticed
    similarity in color patterns between different repellent species
  • Hypothesized that if repellent species look alike, it will be easier for a predator to learn to avoid them all
  • The predator has to learn just one color pattern for all species to benefit
23
Q

Müllerian Mimicry: Heliconius butterflies

A
  • Heliconius butterflies:
  • species living in the same geographic area share
    the same color pattern
  • different geographic areas have different color
    patterns
  • Benson:
  • H. erato and H. melpomene
  • Painted some H. erato individuals to be nonmimetic
  • Survived less well than controls which were also
    painted but remained mimetic
  • Mallet and Barton:
  • Moved butterflies between geographic areas so
    that in a given area they moved in some mimetic
    and some non-mimetic individuals
  • Non-mimetic individuals survived less well
24
Q

Batesian mimicry: cheating by palatable species

A
  • Bates noticed that sometimes, palatable species (mimics) closely resembled distasteful species (models)
  • Predators learned to avoid noxious models and subsequently avoid palatable mimics
  • Mimics survive better the more closely they resemble the model
25
Q

Costs of aposematism

A
  • Bright colors can be costly to ‘wear’ - for example, some coloration can interfere with thermoregulation
  • Bright colors can be costly to make: toxicity often comes from consuming toxic plants, so the toxic animals themselves must maintain defenses against those toxins, which can take energetic resources
26
Q

Costs of crypsis

A
  • Being cryptic might interfere with other behaviors (you want to be conspicuous to mates, for example)
27
Q

Brood parasites

A
  • Exploit the parental care of other species
  • Lay their eggs in a host next
  • Trick other birds into incubating their eggs and raising their young
  • When successful, brood parasites get to have their young raised for free
  • Selection for successful parasitism is strong
  • Successful parasitism results in zero reproductive success for the host –> selection against successful parasitism is strong
28
Q

Hosts fight back against brood parasitism: co-evolutionary arms race

A
  • Cuckoos have evolved in response to hosts
  • Physical appearance mimics that of a bird of prey
  • Egg laying tactics
29
Q

Cuckoo egg laying tactics

A
  • Fast-laying
  • Usually remove one egg when they parasitize a nest
  • They time laying to when hosts would usually lay eggs
30
Q

How have hosts evolved in response to cuckoos?

A

Egg rejection ability:
- Species unsuitable as hosts (wrong diet, nest type, etc.) do not reject cuckoo eggs
- Some hosts show extremely refined ability to discrimintate ‘self’ from ‘parasite’ egg and reject the parasite

Egg ‘signatures’
- Hosts also evolve more complex egg ‘signatures’ that say ‘this is my egg’
- Signatures make mimicry more difficult for the cuckoo, since it is harder to produce a good match to any given host

31
Q

Prediction slide forward

32
Q

Moving on from eggs… why don’t hosts reject chicks?

A
  • Hosts impring upon their own egg type the first time they lay an egg and subsequently reject odd-looking eggs
  • This is not foolproof – sometimes, they reject their own egg if it looks a little odd
  • Impringing on a chick could be too mistake-prone and mistakes too costly:
  • Chicks change appearance each day
  • If you accidentally throw your own chick out, that’s bad!
  • If your brood is parasitized, you would imprint on a parasite chick
  • At the chick stage, better to accept anything than imprint and reject