Emotions, Stress, and Executive Function Flashcards

Lecture 10

1
Q

what are emotions? how do they differ from mood?

A

subjective, conscious experience that is characterized by psychophysiological expressions, biological reaction and mental states

  • emotions influence how we act while moods influence how we perceive
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

are emotions irrational? why or why not?

A

they are not irrational

  • emotions are preserved among mammalian lineage, there is must be an adaptive value
  • parts of the brain responsible for emotion are also responsible for decision making
  • emotion is critically linked to how we value things
  • disruptions that impair emotion also impair judgement and decision making
    • ex. psychopaths are worse at making decisions, don’t consider the right things when making decisions
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

what are the two theories for emotion lateralization

A
  1. right hemisphere model of emotion - right cerebral hemisphere is specialized for emotional processing
  2. valence model - the two hemispheres govern different valences of emotion
    • left = positive emotions
    • right = negative emotions
  • neither theory holds up well but we see small differences between hemispheres
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

why do we sometimes think of emotions as discrete?

A
  • paul ekman argued we have 6 primary emotions that are universal
  • anger, fear, surprise, disgust, happiness, sadness
  • in reality we found little consistency in how cultures rank emotions (except happiness)
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

how do we think of emotions in terms of dimensions?

A
  • think of emotions based on the dimensions of valence (pleasant vs. unpleasant) and intensity
  • dimensions are thought to guide perception and action (approach/avoid)
  • but, we sometimes approach unpleasant things (sad music) and are ambivalent about some things
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

what are the brain regions of emotion?

A
  • brain areas responsible for emotion are diffuse and overlapping
  • population coding - one neuron can’t represent the emotion, the population of neurons firing represents the emotion/feeling
  • it is hard to know if a brain area encodes a value or arousal or emotion
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

what is kluver-bucy syndrome?

A
  • removed the amygdala and part of temporal lobe of animals (anterior temporal lobectomy)
  • animals presented very different behaviour
    1. lack of fear - would approach instead of avoid
    2. hyperorality - explored items by putting them in their mouths (problems with vision and recognition)
    3. misdirected hyper-sexuality
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

how is the amygdala important in emotional learning?

A
  • amygdala monitors the association between two different things (classical conditioning)
    • is a coincidence detector
  • there is a high and low road to fear
    • high road is responsible for learning fear
    • low road
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

how do the high and low roads to fear function?

A
  • high road goes from thalamus to cortex before the amygdala
  • low road goes straight from the thalamus to the amygdala
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

how does damage to the amygdala affect fear (depending on location)?

A
  • damage to lateral amygdala → impairs learning of fear association
  • damage to central amygdala → impairs expression of learned fear
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

who is patient SM and how did they experience fear?

A
  • had a disorder where they had complete bilateral amygdala loss
  • had no experience of fear and no learned fear conditioning
    • often approached fear stimuli instead of avoiding
    • interpreted arousal as excitement instead of fear
  • could still experience fear under the right conditions, like when exposed to CO2 inhalation
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

why could patient SM experience fear during CO2 inhalation but not under any other condition?

A
  • amygdala is used to trigger a state of fear when we encounter threatening stimuli in the external environment
    • fear induced by internal stimuli are not controlled by the amygdala
  • brain stem is responsible for introceptive fear-inducing stimuli
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

how does patient SM represent the negative consequences of not experiencing fear?

A
  • she had difficulty detecting threats and learning to avoid dangerous situations
  • fear is adaptive, the amygdala promotes survival by compelling the organism away from danger using fear
  • learned fear also helps us not make the same mistakes repetitively
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

is the amygdala negatively valenced?

A

no

  • in primate/rodent electrophysiology, the same amount of neurons that fire for aversive stimuli in amygdala, also fire for appetitive stimuli
  • the amygdala is important in changing our values
    • during stimulus de-valuation (satiation) - if you get a good thing repeatedly, we start to de-value it
  • we evaluate appetitive vs. aversive stimuli differently
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

what other parts of the brain are responsible for fear?

A
  • PAG - periacqueductal grey
    • part of the midbrain, tegmentum
    • one of the main recipients of the activity from amygdala
    • fear and shrieking behaviours
  • ACC - anterior cingulate cortex (white matter?)
    • runs next to corpus collusum
    • activated during fear
  • insula - hidden piece of cortex
    • can be seen inside the lateral fissure
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

what is anger driven by?

A
  • anger is largely driven by inequality
    • when fairness is compromised
  • anger is prosocial, ensures people are treated fairly
  • capuchin monkey gets angry after they see that the partner gets a grape while they get cucumber
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
17
Q

what is the ultimatum game and what does it tell us about anger and prosociality? how do we see this reaction in the brain?

A
  • two player game where one play (actually a computer) divides money between themselves and another player, and the other player must accept or reject the offer and neither of them gets money
  • as the split becomes more uneven, people reject the offer more often
  • people say they want to punish the other person in hopes of better behaviour in the future, even at a cost to themselves (altruism)
  • insula and anterior cingulate becomes more activated as more people reject offers (BOLD response)
18
Q

what is a homoeconomist?

A
  • the economic human, we don’t correspond to rational economic behaviour
  • we waste money and do things that don’t lead to best economic outcome
19
Q

what is the purpose of frustration?

A

to motivate us to try to go harder and further in the future

20
Q

what is the near miss effect?

A
  • when you fail but are almost successful
  • results in the largest frustration we can feel
21
Q

how does the near miss effect change behaviours on a slot machine task?

A
  • when you have control of the arm of the slot machine, the near miss makes you more likely to play again (33%)
    • makes us feel we are in control
  • a full miss makes you less likely to play again
  • irrational overall because in a casino, having a near miss doesn’t necessarily mean we are getting closer
22
Q

what does brain activity look like during a near miss?

A
  • anterior cingulate cortex activity goes up more for near misses vs. full misses
  • anterior insula activity is correlated with subjective effects of near miss
  • responses are enhanced in individuals with pathological gambling disorder
23
Q

what is a prefrontal lobotomy?

A
  • procedure previously done to “reduce disruptive behaviour”
  • usually done on people in psychiatric and prison institutions, women, and children
  • put a orbitoclast into the prefrontal cortex and moved it around
  • did not improve disruptive behaviour - some people had drastic changes and others had less
24
Q

what are some executive functions?

A

planning, organization, flexible thinking, monitoring performance, multi-tasking, self-awareness, motivation, determining and exhibiting appropriate behaviour, regulating emotion

25
Q

how does bilateral damage to the ventral prefrontal cortex affect executive function?

A
  • vmPFC is known to regulate emotions
  • lots of other executive dysfunction but most intellectual ability is preserved
  • problems with prioritization, emotional dysregulation, repeating mistakes, planning, rigidity in thoughts/actions, attention and concentration
26
Q

how does unilateral damage to the ventral prefrontal cortex affect executive function?

A
  • most detrimental dysfunction when damage is in the right side
    • disturbances associated with bilateral damage
    • more executive dysfunction
  • people with damage to the left side will lead more normal lives
    • typical social and interpersonal behaviour
    • stable employment
    • personality relatively unchanged
    • decision making ability relatively intact
27
Q

who was phineas gage?

A
  • medial prefrontal cortex had been damaged
    • previously thought that damage was bilateral, but there is more substantial damage in the left side
  • lost fundamental aspects of his personality but still could maintain a job
  • a decade after his injury, he developed severe epilepsy that ultimately led to his death
28
Q

what is passive vs. active stress?

A
  • passive stress - something we need isn’t provided
  • active stress - something happening to us
29
Q

how does controllable vs. uncontrollable stress affect us differently?

A
  • stress immunization - exposure to controllable stress results in more resiliency to stress
  • learned helplessness - exposure to uncontrollable stress results in less resiliency to stress in the future, even if it’s controllable
30
Q

what are the two physiological responses to stress?

A
  1. stress is a sympathetic response - controlled by nervous system signal, moves fast and leaves fast
    • stimulates adrenal medulla and release norepinephrine into the body
    • causes heart to raise faster and blood vessels to dilate
  2. stress is controlled by the HPA axis -controlled by hormones in the bloodstream, slower response that lasts longer
    • hypothalamus signals to pituitary gland that releases hormones
    • adrenal gland releases cortisol
31
Q

is stress beneficial or costly?

A
  • stress is an adaptive response to threat
  • mild levels of stress are beneficial but higher levels of stress have both costs and benefits
  • when under high stress, we perform better in skills that are well-known but worse in skills that are new to us or not well-known
32
Q

what are the benefits and costs associated with high stress?

A
  • benefits: improves implicit memory, simple tasks, habitual and well-rehearsed tasks, and short-term immune system suppression
  • costs: compromises cognitive flexibility, working memory, executive functions
33
Q

how does catecholamine release relate to stress and arousal on the prefrontal cortex?

A
  • there is a linear relationship between how stressed we are and how much catecholamines are released
  • the affects of catecholamine release on performance look like the yerkes-dodson curve (based on arousal)
  • with low levels of catecholamines, the prefrontal cortex fires less, is less likely to work together for a task, and is less discriminatory for what to fire for (easily distractible)
  • at optimal levels of catecholamine release, the neurons create networks to work together and discriminate what to fire for
  • in high stress, prefrontal cortex is suppressed and fires less while other parts of the brain (amygdala/basal ganglia) take on bigger roles
    • striatum is responsible for habitual behaviour and amygdala learns associations
    • high stress improves habitual behaviour
34
Q

what is chronic stress and how does it affect us?

A
  • stress that is constant
  • compromises immune functioning, mental health
  • reduces hippocampal volume, thins PFC
    • reduces performance on hippocampal- or PFC-dependent tasks
  • requires more PFC activity to have same level of performance
    • sends us further to the left of the yerkes-dodson scale
  • shifts more reliance to subcortical structures (amygdala)
  • may decrease baseline dopamine function
  • effects often more pronounced in women
35
Q

what is burnout?

A
  • loss of motivation and pleasure in things that were once enjoyable
  • problems with irritability and looking ahead
  • effects of chronic stress are reversible (restitution)
36
Q

what is the effect of the chronic stress of poverty?

A
  • chronic stress of poverty affects executive functions, cost/benefit decision making, and ability to look ahead
  • poverty affects cognitive performance on unrelated tasks
  • exposure to irregular reward intervals guides individuals to act impulsively
    • even people with high baseline self-control
  • poverty disproportionately affects children and their brains → less grey matter volume
  • poverty is chronic exposure to uncontrollable stress → has an effect on the brain and learned helplessness
37
Q

what is ADHD? what affects diagnosis?

A

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

  • extreme inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity
  • intersects with development, maturity, and societal norms (2x more boys than girls)
  • girls and women more often the inattentive subtype and potentially underdiagnosed
  • youngest children in class are 40% more likely to be diagnosed with ADHD because they’re more immature compared to peers (overdiagnosed)
  • diagnoses have increased more than 100% since 2007
38
Q

how do we treat ADHD?

A
  • psychostimulants (amphetamine, methylphenidate) improve symptoms
  • medications block DAT and NET, increasing functioning of these systems
  • are stimulants but somehow improve hyperactivity
39
Q

how can we explain why psychostimulants improve symptoms of ADHD? what parts of the brain look different in people with ADHD?

A
  • people with ADHD sit further on the left of the Yerkes Dodson curve
  • low levels of catecholamine activity in PFC, firing less often, less integrated, and less discriminatory (more distractible)
  • PET studies suggest DA system is hypoactive, but there are no changes to norepinephrine
  • many studies suggest changes to PFC, DA system, and basal ganglia
    • symptoms of ADHD relate directly to executive function
40
Q

how can we differentiate adult ADHD from chronic stress and burnout?

A
  • all three result in less catecholamine release and sitting further left on yerkes dodson curve
    - symptoms also overlap
  • ADHD may also increase risk of burnout
  • requires nuanced diagnosis