Colour perception Flashcards
Spectra and colour matches
Colour is perceived differently in different species, depending on their opsins - so reflected colour from a real lemon has a different spectrum than a metameric match made using RGB pixels only, which to our eyes looks identical. To an animal with different opsins, these two spectra would look different from each other.
Sunlight and tungsten lightbulbs emit light across a whole spectrum. Fluorescent light bulbs and lasers emit only at one wavelength.
The illuminance spectrum matters, because it is combined with the surface reflectance of an object to produce the colour signal.
It’s all about the /relative/ stimulation of different cone types.
What is colour vision
The ability to detect different wavelengths independent of their brightness
How do we detect colour? (briefly)
visual pigment = rhodopsin plus chromophore
chromophore is the biggest determinant of spectral sensitivity
Young Helmholtz Maxwell theory - trichromacy through three cone types of different sensitivity
Hering colour opponent theory - three cone types into a broadband and two subtracted colour channels - allowing black/white (for intensity), red-green, and yellow-blue.
Grasshoppers use corneal filters as the basis of their colour vision (they only have one visual pigment), and many insects have distal photoreceptors filtering the light for proximal.
How can we prove colour vision in animals?
First, prove they have more than one opsin (although grasshoppers..)
Then, can they distinguish between a coloured object and a black/white object of the same intensity (i.e. reflecting the same number of photons?)
First achieved by Karl von Frisch in 1914 - put a sugar dish on a blue card surrounded by greys of varying intensities. The bee reliably went to the blue card, wherever it was put that trial, proving colour vision. Further, when the bee was allowed to return to the hive, it returned with many more bees, proving communication - von Frisch later proved they communicated via the ‘waggle’ dance.
The bees could not distinguish between the different greys, so von Frisch suggested bees use colour vision only. But Lehrer showed that motion perception in bees is achromatic. So bees use colour vision for specific tasks, and can disregard it for other tasks.
Many animals are the other way around, and use monochromatic vision for learning tasks (because it is higher resolution in most animals), and thus colour vision wasn’t proven in mammals until much more recently.
Experimenters need to adjust intensities used to the species’ spectral sensitivities. They can test these, but sometimes they vary depending on the conditions (goldfish had different sensitivities when learning to recognise a dark object vs a light one), and sometimes they’re subject to chromatic adaptation.
Adaptations of and to colour vision
Trading off spatial resolution:
Nocturnal hawk-moth - Superposition eyes, with large lenses and short focal length, allow UV, blue and yellow light detection under starlight
Helmet gecko - no rods, and longer + more sensitive cones, allow distinguishing between blue and grey under moonlight.
Oil droplets to focus light:
Clear ones [lungfish] increase range of wavelengths that can be discriminated
Coloured ones [chicken] will focus particular colours of light better by interfering with periodicity - they absorb wavelengths of light below a critical value, narrowing the spectral sensitivity of cones function, hence reduction in overlap. This is only useful when there’s sufficient light that noise is not a problem, because it will reduce the overall quantum catch too.
Rhabdomere pigment filters:
Ogawa et al 2013 - the ventral portion of the Colias erate butterfly is sexually dimorphic - females can see red better, because the proximal photoreceptors in one of their three ommatidia types has a sensitivity 40nm lower than in the male, due to different perirhabdomal pigment
Stomatopod (mantis shrimp) has 4 blue filter types to tune 2 UV rhodopsins. This is the most complex eye on the planet, with 12 cone types.
Colour vision defects in humans
38% of Caucasian men have variation in red opsin spectral tuning
1% of men have a missing red cone - protanopes
1% of men have a missing green cone - deuteranopes
0.001% of people have a missing blue cone [not sex-linked] - tritanopes
0.00001% of people are rod monochromats.
Why have colour vision?
Likely evolved to deal with changing illumination conditions, but also:
Enhances contrast
Enhances object categorisation
Allows image segmentation
Improve saliency (more details to make memory trace more unique)
Break camoflage
Enhance own camoflage (note - this doesn’t require conscious processing of colour, e.g. cephalopods)
Improve intra-specific communication (e.g. UV butterflies, UV stripe evolved at same time as duplication of UV opsin)
Improve inter-specific communication (e.g. flowers colours coevolve with pollinator spectral sensitivities, can be species specific)
Allow colour constancy
Improve motion perception
Improve image compression (i.e. you only need certain aspects of a scene/object to recognise it)
Colour ‘vision’ in cephalopods
Cephalopods (octopus, cuttlefish, squid) change colour as camouflage. It was assumed this relied on eyesight. But biopsies of the skin changed colour when a different light was shone on them, independent of brain/eye. They found that the skin responded most quickly to blue light (similar peak sensitivity to the opsin in the eye), then found expression of a gene for an opsin in the skin similar to the opsin in the eye, which was later found to be in the chromatophores themselves. That is, the light-sensing structure is in the colour-changing component.
How do we measure colour sensitivity
Ideal is intracellular recordings, which can be achieved by making an 80 micron hole in the fly cornea and then impaling a single photoreceptor. Can measure voltage change over time exposed to light, and voltge changed over changing light intensity. If you use a monochromator, you can assess spectral sensitivity.
Old-style monochromators - xenon bulb, grating to split it into its component colours, then slit to direct one of those colours towards the animal. Xenon bulb subject to temperature fluctuations, and slow.
New style monochromator - different coloured LEDs in an array, a planar reflective diffraction gating acting as a wavelength combiner, and launching the combined light into a fibre. Only switch each LED on when its input is needed.
Alternatively, electroretinogram - one electrode on the eye, one on the body, recording summed responses of all the cells the light hits. May be hard in animals with many different cone types.
Visual pigment Microspectrophotometry - prepare cryo-sections of retina in the dark (or red light only, to avoid bleaching) and mount on microscope. Bleach with a particular wavelength light, then scan again. For each wavelength, average over many aquisitions, and repeat with many sectioned rhabdoms. Scans normally range 300-600nm, in 1-5nm steps. You’re measuring the absorbance, and estimating spectral sensitivity from that.
You can also estimate from the amino acid sequence
Ogawa et al 2013 used the reflectance spectra from the butterfly tapetum to estimate.
Colour constancy
All insects tested before 2014 showed colour constancy. Important in identifying the same colour under different lighting conditions. Werner’s bee Mondrian showed they can do it, though not all illumination changes were compensated equally well
von Kries thought it could be due to adaptation - they get used to the illuminant light, so all lighting conditions become the same. But Lotto and Chittka showed that bees have colour constancy immediately (not slowly like adaptation), and can detect a colour under conditions A and an opposite colour under condition B - rather than adapting to the illuminant, they were using it. Also, bees alter spatial foraging in patchy illumination to avoid changing conditions!
Computer models found colour blind bees were the worst at foraging, but chromatic weren’t much better. Neither von Kries adaptation, white patch calibration (which assumes brightest part of scene must be white) or grey world assumption (which assumes the average of each channel is the representative grey of the illuminant.) were as good as perfect colour constancy.
Bees have lateral inhibition, in the lamina for example, which may underly it.
Variation in visual pigments
Mammals use A1 derivative chromophores, reptiles and fish use A2, insects use xanthophyll derivatives. Using A2 causes a red-shift from A1, but most of the variation in spectral sensitivities comes from the opsin.
Most animals just use one type of chromophore, but fish and amphibians often combine A1 and A2 derivatives
Mice have two types of chromophore in the same opsin!
Opsin categorisation
Were originally split into r-opsins (found in rhabdomere-type photoreceptors, depolarise via Gq. Vertebrate melanopsins included.) C-opsins (found in cilial photoreceptors, which hyperpolarise via transducin).
Then Eakin introduced the Cnidops (which predated the other two) and Group 4 (a big mish mash, but including the scallop Go linked, which may suggest the beginning of a new evolutionary line).
Remember not all opsins are image-forming! e.g. melanopsin, cephalopod skin opsin.
Levels of colour vision - what are the levels?
1) Spectral discrimination independent of spatial detection (e.g. phototaxis)
2) Spatial and spectral inputs combined (wavelength-specific behaviour)
3) Innate colour preferences that require learning (i.e. animal has a conscious representation of colour)
4) Can use colour appearance (e.g. hue vs saturation) or colour categorisation to make behavioural decisions
E.g. of 1st level of colour vision
Spectral discrimination independent of spatial detection:
Daphnia spp move towards yellowish water. This has been shown to be independent of intensity. Daphnia have 4 types of photoreceptor but no image forming eye. This behaviour is to maintain position in the water column
E.g. of 2nd level of colour vision
Innate preferences for coloured objects:
Doesn’t require conscious colour perception, just ability to discriminate between wavelengths and guide behaviour by that. Similar idea to ‘blindsight’.
Fiddler crabs show sexual preference for longer wavelengths of colour on claw. They approach yellow claws over grey claws regardless of intensity, but find it hard to distinguish between yellow claws of varying intensities.
Papilio butterflies have eight photoreceptor types, but only use three when looking for leaves to lay eggs on, and only one opponent mechanism on these, so are technically dichromat in this behaviour. However, when looking for a mate they are tetrachromatic.