Attatchments (Paper 1) Flashcards
Attachment
Attachment is an emotional bond between two people. It is a two-way process that endures over time.
Developmental Psychology
Developmental psychology is a branch of psychology concerned with the progressive behavioural changes that occur in individuals across their lifespan.
What are the two main aspects of Caregiver-Infant Interactions
Reciprocity and Interactional synchrony
What is Reciprocity
Reciprocity is also referred to as turn-taking. It is a two-way, mutual process, where each party responds to the other’s signals to sustain interaction. The behaviour of each party elicits a response from the other. Studies have demonstrated that infants coordinate their actions with their caregiver’s actions in a kind of conversation. The regularity of an infant’s signals allows a caregiver to anticipate the infant’s behaviour and respond appropriately. This sensitivity to infant behaviour lays the foundation for later attachment between the caregiver and the infant.
What is Interactional Synchrony
Interactional synchrony is when adults and babies respond in time to sustain communication. The caregiver and the infant interact in such a way that their actions and emotions mirror each other. Research has found that infants as young as two or three weeks old imitated specific facial and hand gestures that they saw adults do. An adult model displayed one of three facial expressions or hand movements. A dummy was placed in the baby’s mouth during the display to prevent any response. Following the display the dummy was removed and the infant’s expression was filmed. They found that there was an association between the infant’s behaviour and the adult model.
Strengths for Caregiver-Infant Interactions
Murray and Trevarthen (1985) got mothers to interact with their babies over a video monitor. In the next part of the study the babies were played a tape of their mother so she was not responding to them. The babies tried to attract their mother’s attention but when this failed they gave up responding. This shows that babies want their mothers to reciprocate.
Abravanal and DeYong (1991) observed infant behaviour when interacting with a puppet that looked like a human mouth opening and closing. Infant’s made little response to this, which shows they are not just imitating what they see; interactional synchrony is a specific social response.
Weaknesses for Caregiver-Infant Interactions
Babies cannot use language to communicate so psychologists are relying on their inferences. They cannot be sure that infants are actually trying to communicate with their caregiver.
The expressions tested (tongue sticking out, yawning, and smiling) are ones that infants frequently make so they may not have been deliberately imitating what they saw.
Difficulties Investigating Caregiver–Infant Interaction
- Studies have found that babies’ attachment behaviours are much stronger in laboratory settings than they are in their home environment. Therefore, studies should take place in a natural setting (e.g. the child’s home) to increase validity.
- Most studies into caregiver-infant interactions are observational so there may be bias in the observer’s interpretation of what they see (observer bias). This can be countered by using more than one observer (inter-rater reliability).
- There are practical issues when investigating caregiver-infant interactions. Infants are often asleep or feeding when psychologists want to observe them. Researchers need to use fewer but shorter observation periods because of babies limited waking periods.
- Extra care needs to be taken is relation to ethics when investigating caregiver-infant interactions so as not to affect the child or parent in any way e.g. protection from harm, confidentiality etc.
What are the four Stages of Attachment
Pre-attachment (0-3 months)
Indiscriminate attachment (4-7 months)
Discriminate attachment (7 months onwards)
Multiple Attachments (7 months onwards)
Who studied Stages of Attachment
Shaffer and Emerson (1964) investigated the development of attachment in infants using a longitudinal study where they followed 60 infants and their mothers for two years. They decided that there were four stages in the development of attachment in infants.
What is Pre-attachment
From six weeks of age infants become attracted to other humans, preferring them to objects and events. This preference is demonstrated by their smiling at people’s faces.
Indiscriminate attachment
Infants begin to discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar people, smiling more at people they know. They will still allow strangers to handle them.
Discriminate attachment
Infants develop a specific attachment to their primary attachment figure (usually the mother) staying close to that person. They show separation protest (the distress an infant shows when their primary attachment figure leaves them) and display stranger anxiety (the distress an infant shows when approached by someone they do not know).
Schaffer and Emerson (1964) noticed that the infant’s primary attachment figure was not always the person who spends the most time with the child. They concluded that it is the quality of the relationship, not quantity that matters the most in the formation of an attachment.
Multiple Attachments
Very soon after developing their first attachment infants develop strong emotional ties with other major caregivers, such as the father and grandparents, and non-caregivers, such as siblings These are called secondary attachments. The fear of strangers weakens but their attachment to their primary attachment figure remains the strongest.
Strengths for Stages of Attachment
You thought there was an answer but it was me, Dio!
(No strengths)
Weaknesses for Stages of Attachment
The data collected by Shaffer and Emerson (1964) may be unreliable because it was based on mothers’ reports of their infants. Some mothers might have been less sensitive to their infant’s protests and therefore been less likely to report them.
The sample was biased because it only included infants from a working-class population and thus the findings might not apply to other social groups.
The sample was also biased because it only included infants from individualist cultures, infants from collectivist cultures could form attachments in a different way.
The study does not have temporal validity, it was conducted in the 1960s and parental care of children has changed considerably since then. More women go out to work and more men stay at home.
Stage theories such as this one are inflexible and do not take account of individual differences, some infants might form multiple attachment first, rather than starting with a single attachment.
What where the findings of Schaffer and Emerson surrounding Multiple Attachments
Schaffer and Emerson (1964) found that for the majority of babies their mother is their primary attachment figure. At around 7 months of age babies form secondary attachments to other family members, including their father. 75% of infants studied had formed an attachment with their father by the age of 18 months.
Role of the Father
Fathers are less likely to be their child’s primary attachment figure. This might be because they spend less time with their infants. It is also possible that most men are not as psychologically equipped to form an intense attachment because they lack the emotional sensitivity that women have. This could be due to biological factors. The female hormone oxytocin underlies caring behaviour so women are more orientated to interpersonal goals than men. Alternatively, it could be due to societal norms. In some cultures there is also the stereotype that it is feminine to be sensitive to the needs of others.
What do some researchers believe about the Role of the Father
Some researchers believe that the father fulfils a qualitatively different role from that of the mother. Fathers provide play and stimulation to complement the role of the mother, which is to provide emotional support. The father’s role is considered just as crucial to the child’s wellbeing.
Strengths for Role of the Father
You thought there was an answer but it was me, Dio!
(No strengths)
Weaknesses for Role of the Father
There is inconsistency in the research as to the importance of the role of the father. Research investigating the effects of growing up in a single female or same-sex parent family show there is no effect on development, and therefore suggests the role of the father is not important. However research also questions whether the father plays a distinct role. Studies show that the father in a single parent family is more likely to adopt the traditional maternal role and can be their child’s primary attachment figure.
It seems the father is less important to later development than the mother in terms of nurture. Grossman (2002) found that the early attachment to the mother was a better predictor of what the teenage relationship was like than the early attachment with the father. However, if the father had engaged in active play with the child when they were young, the adolescent relationship with BOTH parents was strengthened.
Field (1978) conducted research which compared the behaviours of primary caregiver mothers with primary and secondary caregiver fathers. Face-to-face interactions were analysed from video footage with infants at four months of age. Overall, it was observed that fathers engaged more in game playing and held infants less. However, primary caretaker fathers engaged in significantly more smiling, imitative grimaces and imitative vocalisations than secondary caregiver fathers. The behaviour of primary caregiver fathers was comparable with that of mothers’ behaviour. This demonstrates that there is flexibility in the role of the father and how men can respond to the different needs of their children.
What was the Strange Situation
This was the methodology used by Ainsworth et al. (1970) to investigate differences in attachments between infants and their caregivers. It was a controlled observation which took place in a room that had been furnished with some toys.
What was the procedure of the Strange Situation
The investigators observed the infants in a series of three-minute episodes;
- mother and baby,
- stranger enters,
- mother leaves,
- mother returns
What was recorded in the Strange Situation
They recorded an infant’s proximity seeking, stranger anxiety, separation protest and reunion joy.
What were the three attachment types found
Insecure-Avoidant, Secure Attachment and Insecure-Resistant
(Type A, Type B and Type C respectively)
What was the percentage breakdown of the attachment types
Insecure-Avoidant / Type A - 20%
Secure Attachment / Type B - 70%
Insecure-Resistant / Type C - 10%
Type A – Insecure-Avoidant
Babies with this attachment style will largely ignore their caregiver and play independently while they explore the room. They show no signs of distress when the caregiver is absent (no separation protest) and continue to ignore them when they return (no reunion joy). The baby is distressed when left completely alone but is comforted by the stranger as easily as their caregiver (no stranger anxiety). The caregiver and the stranger are treated in much the same way
Type B – Secure Attachment
Securely attached babies play happily while the caregiver is present and use them as a safe base while they explore the room and play with the toys. The baby is clearly distressed when the caregiver leaves (separation protest), even if they are not left completely alone, and seeks immediate contact with their caregiver when they return (reunion joy). Their caregiver easily comforts them. The baby is wary of the stranger (stranger anxiety) but accepts some comfort
from them when the caregiver is absent.
Type C – Insecure-Resistant
Babies who have an insecure-resistant attachment to their caregivers are fussy and cry more than other babies. They will not explore the room or play with the toys very much, instead they are clingy. The baby is distressed when the caregiver leaves (extreme separation protest), however they resist comfort from the caregiver on reunion (no reunion joy). They strongly resist the stranger’s attempts to make contact (extreme stranger anxiety).
Strengths of the Strange Situation
The Strange Situation has been replicated many times over the years. It is easy to replicate this study because it had a high level of control and standardised procedures. It has been carried out successfully in many different cultures.
Weaknesses of the Strange Situation
This methodology was developed in the United States and so may be culturally biased. Attachment behaviour that is seen as healthy in the United States may not be seen as such in all cultures. In Germany at this time very few mothers worked (less than 1 in 5) but children were encouraged to be independent and self-reliant. German parents view some of the behaviour exhibited by securely attached infants, such as crying when their mothers leave the room, as being spoilt and so do not reward this behaviour. This is why these children may have shown less anxiety when separated from their mothers and been classed as avoidant.
The validity of some measures has been questioned, for instance, it could be argued that proximity seeking could be a measure of insecurity rather than security.
The strange situation is gender biased as it has only ever been carried out using mothers as the caregiver. Children might be insecurely attached to their mothers but securely attached to their fathers. They strange situation is therefore not measuring a child’s overall attachment style but their attachment to one individual. Main and Weston (1981) found that children behave differently depending on which parent they are with.
The Strange Situation being artificial is that it may not reflect the infant’s real world behaviour (lacks ecological validity). Studies have found that babies’ attachment behaviours are much stronger in laboratory settings than they are in their home environment.
Who studied Cross Cultural Variations in Attachment
Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg (1988) conducted a meta-analysis of 32 studies into attachment to see if attachment occurs in the same way across all cultures.
All of the studies they included had used the strange situation to measure attachment. These studies looked at the relationships between mothers and their babies, all of whom were under 24 months of age. The studies were conducted in eight countries, some individualistic cultures (USA, UK, and Germany) and some collectivist cultures (Japan, China, and Israel).
What were the findings of Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg
The main findings were;
- Secure attachment was the most common attachment style in all of the eight countries studied.
- The second most common attachment style was insecure-avoidant, except in Israel and Japan where avoidant was rare but resistant was common.
- The lowest percentage of secure attachments was in China.
- The highest percentage of secure attachments was in Great Britain.
- The highest percentage of insecure-avoidant attachments was in West Germany.
- Overall variations within cultures were 1.5 times greater than the variation between cultures.
What do the findings of Van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg suggest
The similarities between cultures suggest that caregiver and infant interactions have universal characteristics and so may be partly instinctive. However, the variations between cultures show that the cultural differences in child rearing practices also play an important role in attachment styles. The variations within cultures indicate that sub-cultural differences, such as social class, play an important role in an infant’s attachment style. These factors are possibly more important than culture.