Attachment; Caregiver-Infant Interactions Flashcards
What is attachment?
- A close-two way emotional bond between two individuals who sees the other as essential for their own emotional security
- Endures over time and serves to protect an infant
How do we recognise attachment?
- Proximity - people try to stay close to those they are attached too
- Separation distress - people are distressed when an attachment figure leaves their presence
- Secure-base behaviour - even when we are independent, we tend to make contact regularly with attachment figures; we regularly return to them while playing so they are a base from which to explore
The infant therefore seeks closeness and feels more secure when in the presence of an attachment figure
Caregiver-Infant interactions
- A caregiver is a person who provides care for an infant
- An infant usually refers to a child’s first year of life, although some psychologists also include the second year of life in infancy
- Caregiver-infant interactions refer to the communication between a care-giver and an infant, and it is believed that these interactions have important functions for the child’s social development and form the basis of the attachment between the two
- Particularly, the more responsive or sensitive they are to each other’s signals, the deeper the bond
What is reciprocity?
Reciprocity is a caregiver-infant interaction; from birth, the infant and caregiver spend a lot of time together interacting
- Infants have alert phases and signal that they are ready for interaction
- From around 3 months, these become more frequent and involve close attention to each other’s verbal signals and facial expressions
- This interaction is a mutual process, with each party responding to the signals of the other to sustain the interaction (turn-taking)
- An interaction is reciprocal when each person responds to the other and the behaviour of each party elicits a response from the other, and both the caregiver and infant can initiate these interactions
- Reciprocity is thought to be an important precursor to later communications, and as infant signals are regular, it enables the caregiver to anticipate the infant’s behaviour and respond appropriately; this sensitivity lays the foundation for later attachment
What is interactional synchrony?
- When a caregiver and infant reflect the actions and emotions of the other in a co-ordinated and synchronised way; they mirror each other in terms of their facial and body movements
- This is different to reciprocity as the responses are not necessarily similar
Meltzoff’s and Moore’s Study - 1977
- Observed the beginnings of interactional synchrony in infants as young as two weeks old
- An adult displayed one of three facial expressions or one of three distinctive gestures and the infant’s response as filmed and identified by independent observers using a number of behavioural categories
- The observers did not know what the infants had seen as they were filmed from an angle that meant they could only see the child
- An association was found between the expression or gesture the adults had displayed and the actions of the babies
- In a follow-up study in 1983, they demonstrated this in 3 day old babies which rules out the possibility of this being a learned behaviour and suggests it is innate
- Strengths - controlled, use of angles makes it unbiased, valid, detailed
- Weaknesses - lacks ecological validity as it is an unnatural environment for the mother and infant which could affect behaviour
What is believed about interactional synchrony?
- It is important for the development of mother-infant attachments
- Isabella et al (1989) observed 30 mothers and infants together and assessed a degree of synchronicity, finding high levels of synchrony were associated with better quality mother-infant attachment
Evaluation of Caregiver-infant interactions - Strengths of supporting research and real-world applications
- Observations of mother-infant interactions are generally well-controlled procedures, with both mother and infant being filmed, often from multiple angles which include those that mean the researcher analysing video footage cannot see what the other person in the video is doing - additionally, babies do not know or care that they are being observed
- This avoids demand characteristics or social expectation affecting results as the baby is not trying to impress the researcher - this increases validity
- The use of multiple angles and recording allows observation of results to be analysed repeatedly to find nuances in expression and and the angle itself reduces researcher bias as they cannot see the mother, creating good internal validity - Research into mother-infant interactions is socially sensitive because it suggests that children may be at a disadvantage by particular child-rearing practices, such as mothers returning to work shortly after a child is born, restricting opportunity for achieving interactional synchronicity which is important in developing caregiver-infant attachment
- This suggests that mothers should not return to work so quickly after birth, and therefore has social implications - it could create guilt in mothers who need to return to work in order to generate income, and the research is important as it suggests interactions have a special meaning and may have later implications in life
- These two opposing viewpoints need to be carefully considered when conducting and publishing such research
Evaluation of Caregiver-infant interactions - Weaknesses of supporting research
- (2) Feldman points out that synchrony and reciprocity simply describe behaviours that occur at the same time - they are robust phenomena that can be studied and reliably observed but this may not particularly be useful as it does not tell us their purpose
- It only helps us identify what is happening, rather than why, and so the research is less useful in understanding the use or impact of these interactions beyond assumptions and theorising
- We cannot understand why infants reciprocate and imitate their caregivers, so we cannot be certain they have a special meaning other than acting as an indication of learning - (3) Many studies involving the observation of these attachments have shown similar patterns of interaction; however, what is being observed is merely changes in expression of hand movements and so it is extremely difficult to know what is taking place from the perspective of the infant (are they doing it deliberately?) Other researchers have suggested that infants may instead be showing pseudo-imitation. This is the idea that the infant is not truly imitating behaviour of the caregiver, but are in fact repeating a rewarded behaviour (caregiver laughs and smiles when they imitate) and so arguably the infant has not consciously translated what they see into a matching movement
- Although correlation is being made, causation is not certain - without the intelligence of the babies, we cannot gain a full picture of the cognitive process behind the interactions
- Consequently, we cannot conclude that we fully understand the meaning of these interactions and we can’t know for certain they have a special meaning
Contrasting arguments - evaluation
1 - It is hard to study infant behaviour because their mouths are in constant motion; the expressions being tested in research occur frequently, making it difficult to distinguish between general activity and specific limited behaviours and as such we cannot be certain that the interactions are deliberate and so have a special meaning
2 - There is evidence that reciprocity and synchrony are helpful in the development of mother-infant attachment as well as stress responses, empathy, language and moral development - therefore, it seems they may have a special meaning (know what they contribute too, not necessarily why they do)
3 - Abrabanel and DeYong (1991) observed infant behaviour when ‘interacting’ with two objects, one simulating tongue movements and the other a mouth opening and closing; they found that infants didn’t imitate everything they see and it is a specific social response to other humans and so they have special meanings
The role of the father in attachment
- Infants turn to mothers to seek comfort and nurturing, and they turn to fathers for play
- Fathers are more playful, physically active and generally better at providing challenging situations for their children
- Fathers excite children, encouraging them to take risks whilst still keeping them safe, providing a secure environment to learn to be brave
- Fathers tend to be secondary attachment figures (Schaffer and Emerson 1964)
Why are the roles of the father and mother different?
- It is possible that most men are just not psychologically equipped to form an intense attachment because they lack the emotional sensitivity that women offer
- This could be due to biological or social factors - the fact that fathers do not become the primary attachment figure could simply be the result of traditional gender roles, in which women are expected to be more caring and nurturing than men - fathers therefore feel they cannot act like that, making them more likely to be secondary attachment figures
- On the other hand, it could be that female hormones, such as oestrogen, create higher levels of nurturing and therefore women are biologically predisposed to be the primary attachment figure
The importance of the father - Grossman (2002)
- Carried out a longitudinal study looking at both parent’s behaviour and its relationship to the quality of children’s attachment in their teens
- The quality of infant attachment to mothers, not fathers, was related to children’s attachments as adolescents, suggesting that father attachment was less important
- However, the quality of the father’s play with the infants was related to the quality of adolescent attachments, which suggests that fathers have a different role in attachment, one that is more to do with play and stimulation and less to do with nurturing
The ability of the father to be a primary attachment figure
- There is some evidence to suggest that when fathers do take on the role of being a primary attachment figure, they adopt behaviours more typical to mothers
- Field (1978) filmed 4 month old babies in face-to-face interactions with primary caregiver mothers, secondary caregiver fathers and primary caregiving fathers
- Primary caregiver fathers, like mothers, spent more time smiling, imitating and holding infants that secondary caregiver fathers - this behaviour is suggested to be more important in building an attachment with the infant
- Therefore, fathers can be the more nurturing attachment figure, and the key to attachment relationships is the level of responsiveness, not the gender of the parent
Schaffer and Emerson (1964) - Study Abstract
- A primary attachment figure is the person to whom an infant is most intensely attached and they are the person a child responds to most intensely at separation; they are usually the mother, but others can fulfil the role
- A secondary attachment figure is a person that an infant receives additional support from, providing an emotional support network and safety net
- Schaffer and Emerson found that the majority of babies did become attached to their mother first (7 months) and therefore the primary attachment figure is more likely to be the mother than the father
- They found that within a few weeks or a month of primary attachment, the infants formed secondary attachments to other family members including the father by the age of 18 months
- This was determined by the fact that the infants protested when their father walked away (separation distress) and so they are more likely to be a secondary attachment figure