Agression. Flashcards

1
Q

Agression: The specification details.

A

Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression, including the roles of the limbic system, serotonin and testosterone. Genetic factors in aggression, including the MAOA gene.

The ethological explanation of aggression, including reference to innate releasing mechanisms and fixed action patterns. Evolutionary explanations of human aggression.

Social psychological explanations of human aggression, including the frustration-aggression hypothesis, social learning theory as applied to human aggression, and de-individuation.

Institutional aggression in the context of prisons: dispositional and situational explanations.

Media influences on aggression, including the effects of computer games. The role of desensitisation, disinhibition and cognitive priming.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
2
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Explain the role of the limbic system in aggression.

A

The role of Limbic system in aggression.

Neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression: The limbic system.
The Limbic system – is key area of the brain thought to be involved in moderating aggression. Serotonin and testosterone are also implicate in the behaviour.

The role of the Limbic system:

The Limbic system is a central part of the brain involved in processing emotional responses. It is this area of the brain that is implicated in aggressive behaviour and it is also thought that the works to mediate the level of aggression transmitted from that area.

It is thought that this happens by moderating testosterone levels in response to environmental triggers. More specifically, the amygdala – which is part of the Limbic system, is argued to have an important influence. If the amygdala malfunctioning any way due to a tumour, damage or atypical development then the levels of testosterone can be raised, making aggressive behaviour more likely.

Research support studies:

  • Siegel et al (2009) – reviewed the research on the neurobiological and behavioural explanations of aggression (defensive and predatory). What they found: They found that both forms of aggressive behaviour seem to be controlled by the Limbic system. They also found evidence that the cerebral Cortext played an important role moderating the extent to which aggression was expressed.
  • Raine et al (1997) – scanned the brains of 41 murderers and 41 controls. What they found: they found, using PET scans, that some had abnormalities in the way that their Limbic systems functioned. This suggests that the limbic system is involved in aggressive behaviour.
  • Sumer et al. (2007) – reported on a case study of a 14-year-old girl who, following an MRI scan, was diagnosed with a tumour in the limbic system. This was investigated due to epileptic seizures from the age of three and she was behaving in an aggressive manner, expressing anger and rage. What they found: When the tumour was treated with drugs, the patient returned to normal levels of aggression. This seems to suggest that the high levels of aggression could have been prompted by the tumour in the Limbic system.

The limbic system comprises a set of structures including the:
· hypothalamus,
· the hippocampus
· and the amygdala.
One of its functions is concerned with processing emotional responses, which can be moderated by the cerebral cortex.
The amygdala is extremely important in moderating aggressive responses and many studies have identified its role in aggressive behaviour.

Despite the evidence for the involvement of the limbic system in aggressive behaviour, the fact that the limbic system is composed of many elements makes it difficult to say if a particular area is involved or if there are interactions between several areas. – Also the evidence is only correlational and so it is not possible to say for certain what is cause and what is effect.

Brief evaluation:

· The role of the Limbic system in aggressive behaviour is not clear cut. The Limbic system is made up of many components so it is not altogether clear which parts may be implicated. – it could equally be that there is an interaction between components of the system.

· The research linking brain abnormalities to violence is only correlational (as mentioned above) – this means that there is only an indication of aggression. There are also people who have Limbic system abnormalities who not demonstrate violent behaviour and so causation cannot be ascertained.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
3
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Outline the role of the neurotransmitter serotonin in aggression.

A

The role of the neurotransmitter serotonin in aggression.

Serotonin is a neurotransmitter in the brain which appears to have a widespread calming inhibitory effect, especially involving the pre-frontal cortex. It generally slows down and dampens neuronal activity.

Studies carried out on crayfish (Huberet al.,1997) have shown that those given additional serotonin delayed their withdrawal from fighting compared to those with normal serotonin levels. This could have been because their level of aggression was increased or because their natural responses were overridden. However, a crayfish is a crustacean, and so far removed from a human being, both physiologically and in its behavioural responses.

Studies on human beings have shown mixed results as to the involvement of serotonin in aggressive behaviour. As you look through the evidence, do bear in mind that the evidence for the role of serotonin is complex in that some studies show that high levels of serotonin increase aggression while some show the opposite.

Low levels of serotonin could be responsible for individuals being less able to resist the impulse to be aggressive. Brownet al.(1982) tested the cerebrospinal fluid of people exhibiting high levels of aggression and found that they tended to have low levels of waste products of serotonin, indicating low levels of serotonin in the brain.

It is thought that low serotonin levels result in a reduction in the control of the limbic system by the frontal cortex. This leads to more aggressive behaviour being displayed.

Even human studies suffer from issues of ecological validity due to the measures of aggression not being as realistic as everyday expressions of aggression.

  • Studies carried out on crayfish .Huberet al.,1997. have shown that those given additional serotonin delayed their withdrawal from fighting compared to those with normal serotonin levels. This could have been because their level of aggression was increased or because their natural responses were overridden. However, a crayfish is a crustacean, and so far removed from a human being, both physiologically and in its behavioural responses.

Brief evaluation:

· The Crockett & Passamonti – study highlights why some people get aggressive when they have not eaten. Thus referred to the term “Hangry”. This could be due in part to depleted level tryptophan, which is the dietary source of serotonin. Foods such as eggs and chickpeas are tryptophan rich.

· Much research into neural and hormonal mechanisms in aggression involves animals, which poses a problem for extrapolating to humans. This research is conducted on animals for ethical reasons, and availability of animals, but there is always an issue of using animal studies to explain human behaviour.

· Using biochemistry to explain aggression I’d deterministic*. There is a limited amount of control of the individual over their biochemistry and so, of aggression is explained this way, it is also saying that the individual does not have much control over how they act. This has implications for the legal system and punishing violent crimes.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
4
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: outline the role of the hormone testosterone in aggression.

A

Testosteroneactivates the subcortical areas of the brain to produce aggression. while cortisol and serotonin act antagonistically with testosterone to reduce its effects.

The role of the hormone testosterone in aggression:

Arguably the most widely known biological factor implicated in aggression is the male sex hormone testosterone.

It has been believed for a long time that an increase in testosterone levels leads to heightened aggression. This makes sense if you think about evolution and the male’s role in defending his territory, his mate and his offspring (we will talk more about this in the next topic).

Testosterone is produced in the testes, and castrated rats have been shown to display reduced levels of aggression. However, there is a difference depending on when they were castrated (Connor, Levineet al., 1969). If this happened when the rats were immature, later injections of testosterone did not reverse the loss of aggression. If they had been castrated as mature adults, though, their aggression could be reactivated by injections of testosterone. This means that there must be a developmental process involved in the sensitivity to testosterone.

There are several explanations as to how testosterone could influence aggressive behaviour.

} The enzyme aromatase, found in the limbic system of the brain, metabolises testosterone into estrogen. If its levels are changed, then the amount of testosterone will also change and this will lead to a change in aggressive behaviour. However, research on mice (Trainor, 2006) has shown a variety of responses to aromatase activity. One complication is that estrogen itself can influence aggressive behaviour. Some studies have shown an increase in aggression, whilst others have shown a decrease.

} The activity of the orbito-frontal cortex of the brain is reduced when testosterone levels are high. This will result in heightened aggressive responses in emotional situations.

} Serotonin activity can be reduced by testosterone and low serotonin levels may increase aggressive responses in humans (although, as you have seen, research findings are not all in agreement on this point).

This brief look at some of the mechanisms involving testosterone show how complicated the process is. It seems that testosterone not only has a direct effect on aggression but also mediates the effect of other chemicals.

The role of testosterone:

Testosterone is an androgen (male hormone). Females also have testosterone in their bodies but in much lower levels. Increased levels of testosterone are thought to be related to increased levels of aggression and of aggressive responses, such as provocation.

If a casual link is to he found then the mechanism behind how testosterone affects aggression levels needs to be documented. – some research on mice has suggested that the enzyme aromatase is implicated in the process.

Aromatase = an enzyme which helps metabolize (or process) testosterone.

· Aromatase is important in metabolising testosterone in the brain and is found in the Limbic region of the brain such as the amygdala. So, if there are reduced levels of aromatase in those areas then the testosterone in the brain will not have enough enzyme for it to activate. This means it will have less effect and the response to emotional situations experienced by the amygdala will not prompt a reaction in the testosterone in the system – thus reduces the likelihood of an aggressive reaction.

·
- Research by Higley et al. (1996) – suggests that testosterone levels are not the only factor in aggressive behaviours. They found that testosterone can affect how aggressive an individual feels but that they will not necessarily act on that feeling.

– Therefore it could be concluded that testosterone levels may underpin the emotional response to a situation but that other factors such as social norms will affect whether feeling aggressive influences the actual behaviour of the individual.

  • · As with the research studies on serotonin, much of the work on testosterone in aggression has been carried out on non-human animals. This leads to problems of validity when generalising to humans. The use of self-report techniques to measure aggression in humans brings with it predictive validity issues.

· There are also correlational problems. Observing changes in testosterone and changes in aggression does notmeanthat the changing testosterone levels are causing the change in aggressive behaviour. It could be that aggressive acts lead to changes in testosterone. There could be other factors (e.g. environmental factors) involved too.

· It is argued that testosterone may be able explain only certain types of aggression. Simpson (2001) reports that testosterone is implicated only in inter-male aggression, but has no effect on other types of aggression.

· It is important to consider the measures used in research. Aggression is measured through questionnaires or responses to hypothetical scenarios and therefore the predictive validity (as mentioned above) is poor. – This means that someone may report high levels of aggression in a fake scenario on paper, but were it actually to occur in real life their response might be more passive. Testosterone can be measured using saliva samples and cerebrospinal fluid, both of which are reliable measures. However, hormonal fluctuations occur throughout the day so test-reset reliability may be low too.

Summarised:

As with the research studies on serotonin, much of the work on testosterone in aggression has been carried out on non-human animals. This leads to problems of validity when generalising to humans. The use of self-report techniques to measure aggression in humans brings with it predictive validity issues. There are also correlational problems. Observing changes in testosterone and changes in aggression does notmeanthat the changing testosterone levels are causing the change in aggressive behaviour. It could be that aggressive acts lead to changes in testosterone. There could be other factors (e.g. environmental factors) involved too.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
5
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Describe genetic factors in aggression, in particular the role of the MAOA gene.

A

Genetic factors in aggression, particularly the role of the MAOA gene.

The MAOA gene, which is sometimes nicknamed the ‘warrior gene’ – is implicated in aggressive behaviour due to excessive amounts of certain neurotransmitters in the system.

When you studied issues and debates in psychology in Unit 7, you looked at socially sensitive research. Here we have a classic example of this. Discovering a genetic factor involved in aggression carries the consequence of judging the guilt of people who carry out aggressive acts.

It is reasonable to look for genetic factors in aggression given therangeof evidence showing that aggressive behaviour is influenced by a variety of biological processes. The production of biochemicals is coded in the genome, as is the structural integrity of the brain. However, the environment affects the expression of the genotype, producing a particular phenotype. No single gene has yet been identified as directly causing aggressive behaviour but there has been evidence to show that the predisposition to exhibit aggression can be inherited.

  • When you studied the genetic explanation for schizophrenia (section 6.2 in your textbook) you looked at evidence from twin studies, family relationships and adoption studies. Similar studies have been carried out to investigate the inheritance of aggressive behaviour.

Adoption studies have shown some similarities between the adopted child and its natural parents, which suggests genetic factors. However, similarities between the adopted child and the adoptive parents suggest that environmental factors are also involved.

MAOA gene.

Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) is an enzyme that degrades neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine in the brain: the more MAOA there is in the brain, the less of these neurotransmitters will be present The MAOA gene controls production of this enzyme. Various forms of this gene have been identified, including one which produces low amounts of MAOA and one which results in high amounts of the MAOA. Mice with a dysfunctional MAOA gene have been shown to exhibit more aggression to ‘intruder’ mice in experiments. We again have the validity issue here of generalising this to humans.

  • Research into genetic profiles of various races of people has provided evidence of different MAOA genes in humans. For instance a study of New Zealand males found that those who had the low-activity version of the MAOA gene (MAOA-L) and suffered abuse were nine times more likely to indulge in antisocial behaviour and aggression.

The ‘Brunner syndrome’ is a description of MAOA deficiency. H G Brunner carried out research into the genotype of men in the Netherlands and their level of aggression.

Classic research: Abnormal behaviour associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A study – H.G Brunner (1995).

The Brunner syndrome was first described in 1993 by H.G Brunner in his paper documenting the behaviour of five men in a family in the Netherlands. He found, through monitoring their urine samples, that they bad an excess of monoamines (noradrenaline, serotonin, and dopamine) in their bodies. This was caused by a monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) deficiency. They possessed a shortened version of the MAOA gene and therefore were not able to ‘mop-up’ excess amounts of neurotransmitters.

· The classic case study by Brunner shows how the MAOA gene can affect families that have the gene present in male members. The fact that for this Dutch family, there was such a high relevance of aggressive behaviour supports the MAOA gene theory.

Web research in context:

In the early 1990s, researchers linked low levels of MAO-A with increased frequencies of antisocial behavior, specifically when individuals had a history of being mistreated during childhood.

Later studies by Guo and colleagues (2008) investigatedMAOAvariants in 2500 American boys in grades 7 to 12, and demonstrated a genetic basis for severe aggressive behavior seen at school. A specific variant of the MAOA gene (VNTR 2R MAOA) was a risk factor of violent delinquency, but only when the boys suffered some other stress, such as family issues, low popularity and failing school.

Research (in depth):

  • Moffit et al. (1992) – performed a study in 442 New Zealand males from birth to age 26, keeping a record of experiences of abuse and the presence of the shortened MAOA gene. It was found that those who had suffered abuse and had the low-activity version of the gene, were nine times more likely to indulge in antisocial behaviour, including aggression.

Genetic Factors in Aggression (research evidence) (Evaluation — AO3)
Strengths (AO3 — Evaluation):

(1) Point:Twin studies provide further support for the genetic explanation of aggressive behaviour.Example:For example, McGue et al (1992) found a correlation of +0.43 for MZ twins and +0.30 for DZ twins on aggression scales. This research demonstrates s stronger link to aggressive behaviour for identical twins.Elaboration:This suggests that genes play some part in aggressive tendencies as MZ twins share more genes than DZ twins, therefore if there was no genetic element to aggression we would not see any difference in the correlation for MZ and DZ twins.This is positive asit suggests genes may at least play a part in aggression and therefore that the cause of aggression is, in some part, biological.

Weaknesses (AO3 — Evaluation):
(2) Point:A considerable amount of research into genetic factors in aggressive behaviour was conducted on animals, such as Lagerspetz’s research on mice.

Evidence:For example, this means that the research only investigates the genetic factors in the development of aggression for animals and fails to generate any information regarding human aggression and links to genetics.

Elaboration:This is a weakness because, the findings from this research cannot beextrapolated (generalised)to humans.Mice and humans are physiologically different and so, although there seems to be a genetic basis for aggression in mice, it doesn’t mean that such findings can be generalised to humans.

You will notice that MAOA deficiency resulted in higher levels of serotonin which in turn was associated with high levels of aggression.

  • The MAOA genehypothesiscan only explain the behaviour of one-third of men since two-thirds of men do not possess the MAOA gene variant which produces less of the enzyme. It is also an inadequate explanation for female aggression because it is rarely expressed in women.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
6
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Identify the advantages and disadvantages of the case study as a research method - such as in Brunners research.

A

The advantages of the case study as a research method.

Case studies are in-depth, detailed investigations of one individual (person or institute) or a small group. Brunner only studied five males in one family, which is why it is described as a case study. As such it has advantages, but also limitations. Think about these and list which you think are the most important of each.

· A significant advantage would be that the study would provide rich detail as it is an in-depth study carried out on a small sample.

· Extraneous variables still may factor into the study and with such a sample small, findings might then not be sufficiently generalizable.

Psych answers:
· You may have listed, as the most important advantage, the fact that the study would provide rich detail due to it being an in-depth study carried out on a limited sample. (It may even be theonly wayof researching a unique situation.) However, as it only investigates a limited sample, it is difficult to generalise the findings to a wider population.
· This highlights the trade-off between the idiographic versus the nomothetic approach. If individuals are the focus of a study, more information can be collected and the quality of the information is high. However there is the substantial risk that the participants are not representative of the general public.
· Despite this, the information gathered from case studies is extremely valuable as long as its limitations are kept in mind. Findings from one case study can refute evidence gathered by other methods that might, themselves, have validity issues.
· You might remember the case of ‘Genie’ which you examined during your
study ofattachmenttheory (Section 4 Topic 4).

This study questioned the notion of a critical age at which attachments and language can develop.
Other limitations of the case study approach include:

There may be a question of researcher bias since thedatais usually qualitative (non-numerical) rather than quantitative.

Reliance on memory is an issue since many case studies ask participants to recall events.

Case studies – summarised from the textbook (*repeat):

Case studies are in-depth, detailed investigations of one individual or a small group. They usually include biographical details, behavioural information and experiences of interest. Case studies allows researchers to examine individuals in great depth.

Explanations of behaviour are outlined in subjective ways, describing what an individual feels or believes about particular issues (ref: For example Koluchova’s (1972), ‘Czech twins’ study – page 133 in textbook).

Overall advantages of case studies:

· Rich in detail ~ case studies provide great depth and understanding about individuals and acknowledge human diversity. Because case studies are about ‘real people’, they have a feeling of truth about them. Information relates to a real person, not an average gathered from many.

· The only possible/ practical method to use ~ case studies allows psychologists to study unique behaviours or experiences that could not have been studied any other way. The method also allows ‘sensitive’ areas to be explored, where other methods would be unethical, like effects of sexual abuse.

You should recall that the advantage ofqualitative data– for example the aggressive behaviour studied by Brunner – is that it often tends to be more true to life and richer thanquantitative data. – The disadvantage is that it often suffers from subjectivity in its measurement and lacks detail.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
7
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Explain the use of content or thematic analysis to interpretqualitative data.

A

The use of content analysis and/ or thematic analysis to interpret qualitative data.

Themain difference between thematic analysis and content analysisis that thematic analysis is a method of qualitative data analysis that can be used with varyingresearch designs, while content analysis is a data analysis method that can be used to analyze bothquantitative and qualitative data.

In research and data analysis, researchers use different methods to analyze the data they gather. Thematic analysis and content analysis are the main data analysis methods most researchers use. But for most upcoming researchers, it can be confusing to clearly differentiate between thematic data analysis method and data content analysis method.

Thematic analysis is a data analysis method to analyze the qualitative and descriptive data a researcher gathers to solve his or her research problem. Once the researcher gathers data, he/she would carefully study the data multiple times in order to spot emerging patterns,themes, sub-themes, etc

Content analysis is a data analysis method that the researchers use to analyze both quantitative and qualitative research. Data content analysis allows the researcher to identify significant data out of a data corpus. When it comes to the world of research, researchers gather data from different sources in varying forms, such as pictures, books, ideas, photographs, papers, statues, ideas, behaviors, etc

Overall strengths of content analysis:

· Ease of application – content analysis is an easy-to-perform, inexpensive research method, which is non-invasive, as it doesn’t require contact with participants.

· Complements other methods – content analysis can be used to verify results from other research methods and is especially useful as a longitudinal tool (detecting trends; changes over time).

· Reliability – establishing reliability is simple as a content analysis is easy to replicate, through others using the same materials.

Overall weaknesses of content analysis:

· Descriptive – content analysis is purely descriptive and so does not reveal underlying reasons for behaviour, attitudes, etc. (The ‘what’ but not the ‘Why’).

· Flawed results – Is limited by availability of material; therefore observed trends may not reflect reality. For example, negative results receive more coverage than postive ones.

· Lack of causality – content analysis is not performed under controlled conditions and therefore does not show causality.

Thematic analysis:

Thematic analysis – a method of qualitative research linked to content analysis, which involves analysing data to identify the patterns within it, such as reporting themes (patterns), with patterns identified though data coding. Ultimately thematic analysis organises, describes and interprets data.

The identified themes become the categories for analysis, with thematic analysis performed through the process of coding which involves six stages:

  1. Familiarisation with the data – involves intensely reading the data to become immersed in its content.
  2. Coding – involves generating codes (labels) that identify features of the data important to answering the research question.
  3. Searching for themes – involves examining the codes and data to identify patterns of meaning (potential themes).
  4. Reviewing themes – involves checking the potential themes against the data, to see if they explain the data and answer the research question. Themes are refined, which can involve splitting, combining or discarding one.
  5. Defining and naming themes – involves a detailed analysis of each theme and creating an informative name for each other.
  6. Writing up – involves combining together the information gained from the analysis.

· Thematic analysis goes beyond just counting words or phrases, and involves identifying ideas within data. Analysis can involve the comparison of themes, identification of co-occurrences of themes and using graphs to display differences between themes.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
8
Q

AG: T1. Biological explanations for Aggression: Summary for Biopsychological explanations of aggression.

A

Summary for Biopsychological explanations of aggression:

The limbic system of the brain – the hypothalamus, hippocampus and amygdala – is implicated in aggressive behaviour.

The neurotransmitter serotonin and the hormone testosterone appear to influence aggressive behaviour, but the relationship in each case is complex.

Low levels of serotonin appear to increase aggression, but research findings are not consistent. For example, in Brunner syndrome, aggressive behaviour is associated with an excess of neurotransmitters due to a genetic defect.

The Brunner study is an example of the use of case studies to producequalitative dataon human behaviour. Although useful in some situations, the case study has limitations as a research technique.

Content analysis provides a means of convertingqualitative data, such as that produced by case studies, toquantitative datasuitable for further analysis.

Studies have shown that testosterone may increase levels of aggression, both directly and through its effect on other chemicals.

Twin studies have provided some evidence for a genetic component in aggression.

The MAOA gene may explain aggression in around one-third of western men who carry the shortened version of the gene.

Aggression is likely to be influenced by environmental factors such as experience of abuse.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
9
Q

AG: T2. Evolutionary/ Ethlological explanations for aggression: Describe the ethological explanation of aggression.

A

Fixed Action Patterns [FAP]

AO1

Lea [1984] analysed FAPs and identified 5 features:

  1. Stereotyped – behavior follows a certain pattern each time.
  2. Universal all the animals in that species use the same type of threat.
  3. Innate: all the animals in that species seem to be born with it and don’t have to learn it.
  4. Ballistic: Once it starts it cannot simply be stopped.
  5. Specific triggers seem to set it off.

Also:
Innate Releasing Mechanisms [IRM]
The Hydraulic Model of instinctive behavior [Lorenz 1950]

Ethology is where we learn about human psychology from studying other animals.
• Conrad Lorenz believed that aggression was an innate adaptive response – something which had evolved in humans and animals to help them survive.

• To see off predators: For example a group of hissing geese can drive off a fox, even though the fox would probably win a straight fight. If the geese survive, then the gene which led to that aggressive response will be passed on.
• To get resources: Lorenz also suggested that much aggression was aimed at members of the same species, when competing for territory or sexual partners, but some animals are so fierce they could easily damage each other when fighting for dominance; Eg. Wolves, Stags, Lions.
This would be maladaptive – bad for the species. Therefore they fight until one backs down, not to the death, just to establish who is stronger and who is weaker. Lorenz observed that most intra-species aggression consisted mainly of ritualistic signaling (e.g. displaying teeth) and rarely became physical.
This creates a society in which each individual knows their place. They have evolved ways of warning others to back off: Dogs bark and snarl, cats hiss, apes beat their chest or wave sticks about.
Niko Tinbergen called these Fixed Action Patterns [FAP]

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
10
Q

AG: T2. Evolutionary explanations for aggression: Explain the role of Fixed Action Patterns [FAP], and the research of Tinbergen.

A

Innate releasing mechanisms (IRMs):

Fixed action pattern (FAP):

When the IRM is triggered by an environmental stimulus, a specific sequence of behaviour is released. This sequence is called a fixed action pattern (FAP) because it is universal and invariant across any one species. These behaviours are unaffected by learning and are ‘ballistic’, i.e. once triggered, the behaviour follows a set course and is not subject to alterations. Each individual behaviour is specific to a particular situation and is a response to a particular sign stimulus.

An example of fixed action pattern (FAP) of behaviour involving aggression is the make stickleback fish. The FAP starts with the nesting behaviour in spring. Establishing territory around the nest is important to the stickleback so any other make sticklebacks that encroach on territory are met with aggressive attacks. However, female sticklebacks are encouraged to stay so that they can lay eggs in the pre-prepared nests.

This behaviour was investigated by Tinbergen (1952) using models crafted to resemble male sticklebacks (with red bellies) and females (with swollen bellies). All male sticklebacks attacked the model designed to look like a make member of the species. – This shows that the behaviour is invariant (all males do it) and it is strong argument for the behaviour being innate.

  • It is argued that fixed action patterns are adaptive because they increase evolutionary survival chances. This means that with regard to aggression in humans it must increase survival chances of it is to be regarded as a fixed action pattern. Ao3: This has proven to be one the issues in applying the theory to humans.

Brief note of evolutionary fitness:

The central concept of natural selection is theevolutionary fitnessof an organism. Fitness is measured by an organism’s ability to survive and reproduce, which determines the size of its genetic contribution to the next generation. However, fitness is not the same as the total number of offspring: instead fitness is indicated by the proportion of subsequent generations that carry an organism’s genes. For example, if an organism could survive well and reproduce rapidly, but its offspring were all too small and weak to survive, this organism would make little genetic contribution to future generations and would thus have low fitness.

If an allele increases fitness more than the other alleles of that gene, then with each generation this allele will become more common within the population. These traits are said to be “selectedfor”.

  • The advantage of studying ethology is that we can look at behaviour in terms ofevolutionary advantage, which is difficult to do with humans who now live in artificial environments. Also, more control can be exercised over non-human animals in the laboratory than when studying human behaviours. (Your textbook gives further explanation on the role of ethology in understanding human aggression at the start of section 13.2).

Monkeys reared in isolation still exhibited species-specific reactions to pictures of other monkeys in various situations. This indicates that these behaviours are innate.

Research support with reference to Eibl-Eibesfeldt:

  • Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1977) – argued a number of patterns of aggression in men are highly ritualised and are similar to behaviour found in animals. He cited warfare as an example. However, he also felt that there is an element of control that we are born with, which prevents us from massacring other ‘tribes’. This suggests, certainly from Eibl-Eibesfeldt’s point of view in the matter, that there is a distinction between the automatic processing of animals and the more considered processing of humans, so the ethological explanation has limited usefulness in actually explaining aggression in humans.

Evaluation of research (Ao3):

· An issue with explaining human aggression this way is that it should confer an evolutionary advantage. It is possible this happens in some circumstances, but in others where, for instance – a female is scared; she will not want the aggressor as her partner.

This reduces their chance of reproductive success and so does not show aggression to be an adaptive mechanism.

The fact that human responses vary in the same situation shows the invalidity of suggesting that humans have FAPs. Also, FAPs are not advantageous to humans as they are to non-human animals.

  • Another problem in this research is that aggression is judged by outcome, i.e. killing or injuring another animal. However, the notion of aggression involves ‘intent’ and to observe one animal killing another does not allow intent to be assumed. Predatory behaviour cannot be said to be aggressive since no feeling of aggression is assessable.

Further research points:

Tinbergen presented male sticklebacks with a series of wooden models of different shapes. The red on the competing males’ underbelly is the stimulus that triggers the IRM that in turn leads to the aggressive FAP. He found that if the model had a red underside the stickleback would aggressively display and attack it, but no red meant no aggression. Once triggered, the FAP always ran its course to completion without any further stimulus.

Breland and Breland found that animals tend to revert to instinctive behavior regardless of training. This would support the FAP theory.
It could be argued that some behaviors are learned in the environment – but maybe not all. Dogs can been trained by hunters, army and police to act in particular ways.

Fixed action patterns are not that fixed – Hunt points out that sequences of behaviors that appear to be fixed and unchanging are greatly influenced by environmental factors and learning experiences – Lowers the validity of the theory as it suggests aggressive behaviors are affected by environmental influences

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
11
Q

AG: T2. Evolutionary explanations for aggression: Explain the role of innate releasing mechanisms.

A

Innate Releasing Mechanisms [IRM]

IRMs are innate mechanisms that monitor drives and prompt behaviours following the presence of a stimulus.

FAP stands for a fixed action pattern. These are innate, fixed sequences of behaviour which are universal for a specific species. The behaviour is prompted by an innate releasing mechanism.

AO1

• Creatures have evolved an instinctive response to certain signs. [Like a red rag to a bull!]
Eg. Male sticklebacks will respond aggressively to the red underbelly of a rival male – but not to a female who does not have the red underbelly.

AO3

Cannot generalise to humans – We should be cautious about making such generalisations, especially to a complex behavior like aggression because humans can act upon free will unlike animals - Human aggression is extremely destructive but we seem to have an element of control (our processing might not be automatic / innate) - Aggression cannot truly be measured in animals because intent is not known and cannot be communicated (may be an act of survival, not aggression).

Innate releasing mechanisms (IRMs):

Konrad Lorenz was one of the earliest scientists to study ethology (the study of animal behaviour). He published a book calledOn Aggressionin 1963 and in it he described the innate drive of aggression. Lorenz likened aggression to other instinctive drives such as that for food. He suggested that aggression was ‘a need’ that had to be satisfied, and that animals had a specific neural circuitry inside the brain (this is the IRM) which monitored the drive.

Fixed action pattern (FAP) in relation to innate reading mechanisms:

When the IRM is triggered by an environmental stimulus, a specific sequence of behaviour is released. This sequence is called a fixed action pattern (FAP) because it is universal and invariant across any one species. These behaviours are unaffected by learning and are ‘ballistic’, i.e. once triggered, the behaviour follows a set course and is not subject to alterations. Each individual behaviour is specific to a particular situation and is a response to a particular sign stimulus.

It is believed that animals have a built-in neural structure (a network of neurons) which, when exposed to specific stimuli (signs or releasers) such as facial expressions, will cause the release of an automatic behavioural response (a fixed action pattern). This inbuilt biological structure or process is called the innate releasing mechanism (IRM). For example, when a dog sees a cat running away from them, they have an instinctive response to chase the cat. When the cat is still, the innate releasing mechanism is not activated; however, it is the cat running that activates the innate releasing mechanism

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
12
Q

AG: T2. Evolutionary explanations for aggression: Describe The Hydraulic Model of instinctive behavior - by Lorenz 1950.

A

The Hydraulic Model of instinctive behavior [Lorenz 1950]

It may be easier to understand and remember the hydraulic model if you compare it to a toilet ! The water level gradually fills up till you flush it - then it has to be filled up again.

AO1

• Lorenz said that all creatures build up a reservoir of Action Specific Energy – you could call it “pent up aggression”. When the Innate releasing mechanisms [IRM] trigger the Fixed Action Pattern [FAP] all the aggression is fired off.
Once it is out of the system the animal is less aggressive again till the level of Action Specific Energy has built up again.

AO3

This explanation was probably an example of Lorenz trying to adapt Freudian ideas to animals! Freud wrote about the build-up of sexual energy [Libido] and Lorenz applied a similar idea here.

This theory fails to explain premeditated aggression and bearing grudges.
Holst [1954] found that instead of getting it out of the system , aggressive action could feed back to make the person more angry and increasingly more aggressive.
Arms et al. [1979] found that watching violent sport did not flush aggression out of the system but tended to increase it. Bushman does not agree with idea ofCatharsis– says that aggression may lead to more aggression.

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
13
Q

AG: T2. Evolutionary explanations for aggression: Describe and Evaluate the evolutionary explanation for aggression. (A01/A03).

A

Evolutionary explanations of aggression.

Evolutionary Explanations of Human Aggression.

Explains aggression through natural selection (survival of the fittest, aggressive genes are passed on to subsequent generations as aggressive individuals more able to compete for resources)
The central idea of this topic is that for aggression to be an adaptive feature it has to serve a purpose.

Aggression is Adaptive

AO1

• David Buss has identified 7 adaptations of aggression in humans:
• Self Defence
• Reputation to ward off future aggression
• To achieve status – more allies less enemies
• Get and keep better share of resources. Pinker (1997) states aggression evolved in men to compete for women. This may have been the MAIN reason for aggression as there was no other property worth fighting over as we evolved.
• Deny own resources to children of rivals
• To prevent other males sharing the prime females
• Prevent partner being unfaithful. For example, sexual jealousy may have evolved to ensure that men pass on their own genes rather than allowing other males access to their mate.

Inter-Group Aggression

This is aggression between different groups, such as warfare and gangs.

AO1

•Bussstates human males have evolvedcognitive biastowards organised aggression: E.g.

• Cognitive bias to expect attack
• Cultivating tough reputation
• Use of vengeance as a deterrent
• Strategies for planning and timing an attack
• Deception and the ability to detect deception
•Cosmides and Tooby,the Military Contract: Men will only fight if those who share the rewards also share the danger. Other animals are not bright enough to work this out.

Intra-Group Aggression

This is aggresion within a single group, mainly linked to male rivalry and sexual jealousy.

AO1

• Daly and Wilson: Male – Male aggression among young men is common in all human cultures – suggesting it is evolutionary.
• Pinker (1997) suggests aggression evolved in men to compete for women. This may have been the MAIN reason for aggression as there was no other property worth fighting over as we evolved. Through most of evolution there was no money, no real property, so women were the only target of aggression.
• Potts and Hayden (2008): War and aggression aimed to control women’s mating habits since development of farming made inheritance of land important. Jealousy has evolved as a male response to the threat of infidelity. Jealous males are determined to pass on their OWN genes.
• Daley and Wilson (1988): Men may use jealousy and violence to control partners sexual behavior Violence is not intended to kill but may have that result. E.g. Fertile young women 10 times risk of domestic violence.

The evolutionary explanation depends on the premise that aggression has an important function both in terms of individual survival and reproductive success.
Men being aggressive to other men might serve to show off their attributes to possible mates. This may lead to greater reproductive sucess, furthering their genetic influence on future generations. This is similar to stags displaying during the rutting season.

Exhibiting the ability to fight for resources might make a male not only more able to survive themselves but appear more attractive to females since they can provide for their offspring.

Sensitivity is also important to females, however; if she saw this in a prospective male partner, she might feel they would be nurturing to her offspring, whereas she might feel her offspring could be in danger from their father if he were aggressive.

Jealousy can give rise to aggression due to a perceived potential threat to the relationship. This can confer anevolutionary advantagesince the aggressive behaviour can protect the relationship. Furthermore, aggression can prevent infidelity, which can pose a potential threat to the paternity of offspring. This has been supported by research showing that males are more violent towards partners who are about to leave them.

Research has shown that females are attracted to more dominant males, but do not necessarily think that such men are more likeable. They appear to put their own and their children’s survival ahead of their own happiness.

The evolutionary explanation:

The evolutionary explanation suggests that aggression serves an important function in terms of both individual survival and reproductive success. It can help an individual to fight for resources even if they are in short supply. It might also be, in the case of male aggression, that they appear more attractive to females as they would appear strong. Evolutionary psychologists argue that this would be attractive to women because they would be able to protect offspring.

Evolutionary theory also argues that humans have a natural tendency for aggression and this goes some way to dominate over other species.

The idea of men aggressive with other men is similar to the rutting of Stags, in that it is a chance to display attributes and especially show off to females. The victor is more likely to gain a mate or mates to reproduce with. – This means that they will father more offspring and have a greater chance of furthering their genetic influence on subsequent generations.

  • There is much evidence to back up these claims, coming mainly from studies forced-choice, hypothetical scenarios.

Research support:

  • Sadalla et al. (1987) – suggested that women are attracted to male dominant behaviours, supporting the idea that aggression in men can increase their chance of reproductive success. Interestingly, though, dominant behaviour may have enhanced their attractiveness, but it did not increase how much they were actually liked. It is possible that this shows that mate preference is based on survival rather than happiness.

Section 4: Evolutionary Explanations of Human Aggression.

Explains aggression through natural selection (survival of the fittest, aggressive genes are passed on to subsequent generations as aggressive individuals more able to compete for resources)
The central idea of this topic is that for aggression to be an adaptive feature it has to serve a purpose.

Aggression is Adaptive

AO1

• David Buss has identified 7 adaptations of aggression in humans:
• Self Defence
• Reputation to ward off future aggression
• To achieve status – more allies less enemies
• Get and keep better share of resources. Pinker (1997) states aggression evolved in men to compete for women. This may have been the MAIN reason for aggression as there was no other property worth fighting over as we evolved.
• Deny own resources to children of rivals
• To prevent other males sharing the prime females
• Prevent partner being unfaithful. For example, sexual jealousy may have evolved to ensure that men pass on their own genes rather than allowing other males access to their mate.

Inter-Group Aggression

This is aggression between different groups, such as warfare and gangs.

AO1

•Bussstates human males have evolvedcognitive biastowards organised aggression: E.g.

• Cognitive bias to expect attack
• Cultivating tough reputation
• Use of vengeance as a deterrent
• Strategies for planning and timing an attack
• Deception and the ability to detect deception
•Cosmides and Tooby,the Military Contract: Men will only fight if those who share the rewards also share the danger. Other animals are not bright enough to work this out.

Intra-Group Aggression

This is aggresion within a single group, mainly linked to male rivalry and sexual jealousy.

AO1

• Daly and Wilson: Male – Male aggression among young men is common in all human cultures – suggesting it is evolutionary.
• Pinker (1997) suggests aggression evolved in men to compete for women. This may have been the MAIN reason for aggression as there was no other property worth fighting over as we evolved. Through most of evolution there was no money, no real property, so women were the only target of aggression.
• Potts and Hayden (2008): War and aggression aimed to control women’s mating habits since development of farming made inheritance of land important. Jealousy has evolved as a male response to the threat of infidelity. Jealous males are determined to pass on their OWN genes.
• Daley and Wilson (1988): Men may use jealousy and violence to control partners sexual behavior Violence is not intended to kill but may have that result. E.g. Fertile young women 10 times risk of domestic violence.

General Criticisms of Evolutionary Research

AO3

Ethics:Waller says : Violence ,Xenophobiaand even genocide are adaptive, but this is very deterministic and unethical.
Ethics and Gender: Critics feel this theory could be used to justify violence against women. Buss himself always points out that we are not controlled by our genes, we have inherited the ability to learn and to choose.
Reductionist: Is this an over-simplification? Are there other issues which promote aggression such as culture or Individual differences in testosterone and cortisol.
Heredity & Environment: Are environmental factors a greater cause of aggression?

• Environmental stressors, heat, noise etc
• Cortisol levels in pregnant mother
• Childhood abuse and neglect
Deterministic: Evolutionary explanations may seem to suggest that aggression is natural but Figuerdo [1995] suggests jealousy and domestic violence are context specific not inherent, women are less likely to be victims of domestic violence if they have several brothers in town, so aggression can be controlled.
Socially sensitive– It suggests that aggression is somewhat excusable and out of the control of the individual committing the behavior – This has important consequences within the legal system – Brings into question the credibility of the theory and makes it imperative that such socially sensitive research is not released into the public domain

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
14
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Describe/ recap the SLT approach to explaining behaviour, including Banduras research.

A

Social-Psychological explanations of Aggression.

Social learning Theory

In the 1960s Social learning theory seen as a challenge to behaviorists Suggested children learn things even without doing them, throughobservational learningand modelling.
Exam Tip: If the question asks about Social learning Theory it is not enough only to write about theBobo Doll experiment. That was only one experiment – not the whole theory.

AO1

Children learn aggressive behaviors through observing aggressive models. These may be live models such as parents or symbolic models such as characters in the media.
• Behaviorists believe learning occurs through experience followed by either punishment or reward. “Social Learning Theory” challenges that approach.
• The central idea of social learning theory is that people do not need rewards to learn aggression, they may copy the behavior of others, but this is less likely of they see the other people being punished.
Bandura states children learn by imitation, and are more likely to copy depending on:

The actual behavior of the role model

The status of the person copied

The closeness / immediacy of the person

How well we understand what is happening

Bobo Doll experiments: Children copied adults
Contributory factors:

Similarity: boys will copy boys, family links and groups etc.

Presentation: How close, live, immediate the violence was

Warmth: If the model was more friendly towards the subject

Prestige: If the model had high status

Appropriateness: If the behavior was “appropriate.

•Vicarious reinforcement: (i) Adult was rewarded children slightly more likely to copy; (ii) adult was punished children were much less likely to copy.
•Disinhibition: People are more willing to do things if they see that others are already doing them.
•Bandura’s conclusions: Aggression is not inevitable. Children observe aggressive behavior in others, but how they act may depend on what the consequences of aggression were, particularly for those they use as role models

How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
15
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Explian the frustration-aggression Hypothesis.

A

The frustration-agression hypothesis of aggression.

The frustration–aggression hypothesis.

This theory for aggression is based on the work of Dollard et al. They suggested that aggression is a consequence of feelings of frustration. Frustration is the feeling you experience when you are trying to achieve something, I.e. working towards a goal, and there are barriers (real or imaginary) that are preventing you from realising your aim. It is an unpleasant feeling and needs to be relieved. Aggression, the theorists argue, allows that relief to happen.

You almost certainly know what it feels like to be frustrated and that this can result in you feeling angry. In the case of the man whose garage was blocked by the car of a thoughtless driver, you can certainly feel his frustration. Maybe you can even excuse his actions, even if the law doesn’t.

Dollardet al. (1939) explored this idea and discovered a number of factors which resulted in this frustration being more likely to lead to aggressive behaviour:

The proximity to your goal: the nearer you are to achieving your goal, the more likely you are to become angry if something or someone prevents its achievement.

If the aggression will serve a purpose: certainly the man in the scenario above achieved his goal by taking out his frustration on the parked car.

If the blockage to the goal is deemed unreasonable: if the frustration is caused by a situation that can be rationalised, then you are less likely to get angry and exhibit aggression.

Research support for the frustration-aggression hypothesis:
- There has been extensive research carried out into the factors that turn frustration into aggressive behaviour. For instance, if someone jumps a queue and you are close to the front you are much more likely to react aggressively than if you are at the back of the queue (Harris, 1974).

  • Pastore (1952) – explored justified frustration and showed that aggressive behaviour was lower when the frustration was unjustified.
  • Jody Dill and Craig Anderson (1995) – used an origami exercise to explore the consequence of justified frustration. They had a confederate demonstrate the use of origami to make a complex paper model. In one scenario the demonstrator rushed the demonstration and excused this by saying he had to meet his girlfriend. In another situation the reason given was that the boss had told him he had to finish quickly. Participants were made angrier towards the demonstrator by the situation of meeting his girlfriend than when the boss was exerting pressure. A control group who were not frustrated reported less anger than both experimental groups. The measure of aggression was negative judgements made of the demonstrator.
  • Leonard Berkowitz – believed that frustration might make us feel angry but that aggression will only be caused if other factors exist, such as environmental cues. A famous study was carried out into the ‘weapon effect’. Berkowitz and LePage (1967) – placed two guns next to a table where there was a shock generator. A confederate gave the participant electric shocks and then the participant was allowed to retaliate. The participant was more likely to apply a larger number of shocks in the presence of weapons compared to the situation of no weapons being present. This has led to a famous quote, ‘The finger pulls the trigger, but the trigger may also be pulling the finger.’
    • Buss (1963) – examined whether different types of frustration would affect the likelihood of aggression occurring. He used three different types of frustration:

(1) Failure to win some money.

(2) Failure on a task.

(3) Failure to get the grade you expect.

He found that all types of frustration prompted aggression and all three types prompted higher levels of aggression than a group that acted as a control group with no frustrations, thus supporting the theory.

+ Application to intergroup relations:

· In the realm of intergroup relations, the frustration-aggression hypothesis was used to shed light on thedynamicsofstereotyping,prejudice, and out-group hostility. The theory of scapegoating is probably the most well-known application of the frustration-aggression hypothesis to the study ofprejudice.

· Drawing in part onFreudianconcepts of displacement, projection, andcatharsis, the scapegoating theory held that once frustration and theimpetusfor aggressive behaviour have occurred, it makes relatively little difference who receives the brunt of theviolence.

· In some cases, aggression naturally takes the form of retaliation against the initial source of frustration. In other cases, situational constraints can prevent a person from being able to react against the actual source of frustration (such as when the frustration was caused by a very powerful person or group). In still other cases, such as natural disasters, there may be no one to blame, but the frustration can still produce aggressive inclinations.

· According to the theory, thedisplacementof aggression onto a socially sanctioned (i.e., convenient) victim group serves several purposes. First, and most important, it channels the expression of aggressive impulses and createscatharticrelief once the aggression has been released. Second, it is socially undesirable to behave violently toward others in the absence of justification, but prejudicial attitudes can be used to justify (or rationalize) the expression of hostility. In that way, members of disadvantaged groups can be blamed for their own plight as targets of hostility and prejudice. Finally, in accordance with psychoanalytic thought, the theory of scapegoating suggests that victim blaming isexacerbatedby the projection of (typically unconscious) guilt that frustrated parties feel as a result of their own prejudice and violent activity.

Evaluation of the frustration–aggressionhypothesis:

· Not all frustration leads to aggression and so it matters that the source of the aggression is considered.

· Not everyone will react aggressively given the same situation: some people’s response is to cry or get despondent, so personality and personal experiences must be taken into account. Social learning theory is the subject of the next part of this topic and it will show how personal experiences can influence our behaviour. This raises the issue of nomothetic versus idiographic explanations (Unit 7 Topic 8).

· There are causes of aggression other than frustration and so the frustration–aggressionhypothesisdoes not explain all aspects of aggressive behaviour.

· There are problems of validity with many of the studies into aggressive behaviour, since it is unethical to create actual aggressive behaviour in humans. For example, in one of the studies above the measure of aggression was a judgement about the demonstrator. Very often the measure of aggression is a person’s response to hypothetical situations and in reality they might behave very differently. – This has low predictive validity because in reality they may act qualitatively different.

· Finally, the nature of the connection between perceived frustration and the display of violence also turned out to be more complicated than Dollard and his collaborators realized. In the most empirically successful modification of the original frustration-aggression hypothesis, the American social psychologist Leonard Berkowitz suggested that frustration is a psychologically aversive state that can create a predisposition to behave aggressively. According to Berkowitz, frustration will lead to aggression to the extent that it elicits negative emotions. Moreover, frustration is only one form of unpleasant negative affect that can provoke violent responses.

  • The general idea was that aversive experiences produce negative emotions and feelings, as well as related thoughts and memories of past reactions to negative events. Berkowitz noted that such negative emotions and thoughts lead automatically to thefight-or-flight response. The choice between “fight” and “flight” was thought to depend on the intensity of the negative emotion as well as on the subjective appraisal and interpretation of the situation.
How well did you know this?
1
Not at all
2
3
4
5
Perfectly
16
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Explain how social learning theory may be applied to aggression.

A

How social learning theory may be applied to aggression.

The basic assumptions of social learning theory (SLT)/ recap of SLT:
SLT states that aggression is learnt in the same way as other social behaviours. The main idea is that behaviours of role models are copied (or imitated) if certain mediating cognitive processes are present:

Attention: The individual needs to pay attention to the behavior and its consequences and form a mental representation of the behavior. For a behavior to be imitated, it has to grab our attention. We observe many behaviours on a daily basis, and many of these are not noteworthy. Attention is therefore extremely important in whether a behavior influences others imitating it.

Retention: How well the behavior is remembered. The behavior may be noticed but is it not always remembered which obviously prevents imitation. It is important therefore that a memory of the behavior is formed to be performed later by the observer.

Much of social learning is not immediate, so this process is especially vital in those cases. Even if the behavior is reproduced shortly after seeing it, there needs to be a memory to refer to.

Reproduction: This is the ability to perform the behavior that the model has just demonstrated. We see much behavior on a daily basis that we would like to be able to imitate but that this not always possible. We are limited by our physical ability and for that reason, even if we wish to reproduce the behavior, we cannot.

This influences our decisions whether to try and imitate it or not. Imagine the scenario of a 90-year-old-lady who struggles to walk watching Dancing on Ice. She may appreciate that the skill is a desirable one, but she will not attempt to imitate it because she physically cannot do it.

Motivation: The will to perform the behavior. The rewards and punishment that follow a behavior will be considered by the observer. If the perceived rewards outweigh the perceived costs (if there are any), then the behavior will be more likely to be imitated by the observer. If the vicarious reinforcement is not seen to be important enough to the observer, then they will not imitate the behavior.

SLT, proposed by Albert Bandura, emphasizes the importance of observing, modelling, and imitating the behaviour’s, attitudes, and emotional reactions of others. Social learning theory considers how both environmental and cognitive factors interact to influence human learning and behavior.
In social learning theory, Albert Bandura (1977) agrees with the behaviourist learning theories of classical conditioningandoperant conditioning.

However, he adds two important ideas:

  1. Mediating processes occur between stimuli & responses.
  2. Behavior is learned from the environment through the process of observational learning.

Social learning theory and human aggression.

Research evidence:
There have been a number of studies on SLT and aggression and some of these are described in your textbook, including the following studies on the role of media and the environment:

  • A study carried out by Pinta da Mota Matoset al.(2011) – found that identification with an aggressive model from the television was linked to aggression in Portuguese students, suggesting that the process of social learning affected the level of aggression. What this shows: This supports the use of social, earing to explain some instances of aggression (see contemporary research in textbook, page 575).
  • A video gaming study by Cooper & McKay. (1986) – seemed to show that females are more affected by observing aggressive behaviour. What this shows: This suggests a gender difference in how young individuals react to the social learning of aggression.
  • Williamset al. (1981) – showed that the introduction of television to a remote area coincided with an increase in both verbal and physical aggression. What this shows: This suggests the media to be a source of aggressive behaviour to children as observers.
  • In a study of 4458 children living in urban neighbourhoods, Guerraet al. (2003) – showed that children imitated violence that they witnessed locally. What this shows: This suggests that there is a long-term effect on the behaviour of children exposed regularly to violence in their environment.
  • Bandura. (1977) – found that people who lived in areas with high crime rates have a greater likelihood of committing a violent crime than those who live in low-crime areas. What this shows: This supports SLT as there was exposure to a greater number of aggressive acts in high-crime than low crime-areas, although other factors, such as stress and poverty, can also affect crime figures.

Evaluation of the application of SLT to human aggression:

There is significant evidence that aggression is learnt from observing role models; however, there is doubt as to whether this is long-lasting.

Pacifist behaviour can be learnt from the behaviour of people around; this is thought to occur within the Amish community, for example. This adds extra evidence for SLT being an explanation for behaviour, whether it is pro- or antisocial. If aggression were innate then it would override a belief-based philosophy such as pacifism.

SLT explains why aggression is displayed in certain circumstances. If aggression were solely biological in origin then it would not be so situation-specific.

17
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Explain the process of de-individuation.

A

De-individuation is a social psychological theory that sees aggression occurring when an individual is part of a group or crowd. – It states that aggression is more likely to happen when an individual experiences a loss of their sense of identity. As they no longer feel autonomous as an individual, their morals and beliefs that normally guide their behaviour are loosened and they adopt the morals of any group that they may be part of at that particular time.

It accounts for why someone may become part of an angry mob an aggressive and ‘out of character’. An example would be a football fan who gets involved in fight against the fans of the opposing team following a game. This person may not show any aggression for the rest of the week, but because they lose their identity temporarily when they become part of a crowd of fans they start to act with uncharacteristic aggression.

  • The word ‘de-individuation’ was first used by Festinger et al. To describe the phenomenon in 2952 and there has been much subsequent research examining the idea.

Psychologists revisions:

There are psychologists who believe that crowd behaviour occurs for more complex reasons. Zimbardo (1970) claimed that in addition in to losing anonymity there are influences of drugs/alcohol, a reduced sense of responsibility and sensory overload. The influence of a crowd can have various effects on an individual’s behaviour.

The idea of de-individuation was refined further by Diener in 1980. He felt that de-individuation was mediated by self-awareness. When someone is in a non-crowd situation their self awareness acts as a regulator of their behaviour. However, in a crowd situation, the focus is outward, as there is lots of stimulation to attend to. This shifts the focus outwards and their self-awareness declines. This means that the regulator of behaviour is weakened and uncharacteristic behaviour is likely to occur.

Activity 2

In September 2008 a young man jumped from the top of a city centre car park in Derby. Police negotiators had been trying to convince him not to jump for three hours. However a crowd of around 300 people had gathered and there were many taunts aimed at the man encouraging him to jump, such as ‘How far can you bounce?’ A superintendent in the Derbyshire Police said it was a ‘disturbing and shocking reflection on society when people feel inclined to do that’.
Why do you think people would behave in this way? Write down your thoughts on this incident.

My answers:

· I think that this ‘group mentality’ is certainly something that resonates quite deeply within the human psyche, as an innate tendency. As we see things effect in hunting animals such as dogs and wolves – we see their whole mentality change when they are together as a group, and become far more spurred on by aggressive behaviour and less liable to control.

· I believe that this is almost exactly the same for humans. Especially in a crowd as large as 300 people.

Psych answers:

You are probably shocked by the behaviour exhibited by this crowd. You may also find it very difficult to explain. It appears that when a person is an anonymous member of a crowd they lose their sense of identity and responsibility for their actions and adopt the morals of the group they are part of at that particular time. This allows them to display a level of aggression which is normally frowned on by society and they feel less personal guilt at harmful aggression directed at other people.

The first instance of this sort of behaviour being examined was by Le Bon in 1895 when studying the French Revolution, and the term ‘deindividuation’ was first coined by Festinger et al. in 1952, who looked into how people’s impulses, which are normally moderated by internal standards, rise to the surface.

In the Unit 3 Topic 1 you studiedconformityto social roles and learnt about the work of Zimbardo, so it should not be a surprise to you that he is an influential researcher into social behaviour. Zimbardo (1970) (see above also) believes that crowd behaviour is very complex. Apart from anonymity, several factors have been identified as being influential in crowd behaviour, for example:

drugs/alcohol

darkness

wearing of a uniform

masks and disguises (think of the white robes and masks worn by the Ku Klux Klan)

In his famous ‘prison study’, Zimbardo gave his participants uniforms and they soon ‘played’ the role that the uniform dictated (guard or prisoner).

Self-awareness and Research and evaluation.

The suggestion is that there is a loss of self-awareness when in a crowd; because there is a lot to pay attention to, the focus on attention is outward and so self-awareness declines.

Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1982) describe two types of self-awareness:

public, involving the appraisal of your behaviour by other people

private, where one is aware of one’s own feelings and thoughts.

The causes of deindividuation:

Some deindividuated situations can reduceaccountability, because people who are hidden within a group cannot be easily traced or blamed for their actions. Thus, the effects of deindividuation are sometimes viewed as socially undesirable (e.g.,rioting).

However, research has shown that deindividuation also strengthensadherenceto group norms. Sometimes those norms conflict with the norms of society at large, but they are not always negative. Indeed, the effects of deindividuation can be rather inconsequential (e.g., “letting loose” on the dance floor) or even positive (e.g., helping people).

Modern theories have applied and extended early principles to understand people’s behaviour in smaller groups and in othercontexts, such as when people have the opportunity to interact with others while concealing their identity and remaining anonymous.

Following Le Bon, Festinger and his colleagues proposed that being deindividuated (in particular within a group) reduces normal constraints on behaviour and encourages people to do things they normally would not do, because they are not directly accountable for their actions. They are in a sense liberated to do what they like.

Festinger found support for that idea by demonstrating that participants who engaged in a group discussion about their parents while being dressed alike in a dimly lit room were more likely to make negative comments than were participants in acontrol group. In other words, the deindividuated situation allowed participants to express views that they would normally keep to themselves.

The effect that being in an crowd has on self-awareness:

The more the person becomes involved in the group, the less self-awareness they have, which includes knowing their morals, characters, and beliefs. These qualities start to be replaced by the identity of the group. The individual then begins to take on the morals and character that is held by the group as a whole. Think of being surrounded by fans at your favourite team’s home game.

Everybody’s yelling, some people are thumping their chests, and you’re jumping up and down. Most likely, you wouldn’t exhibit these behaviour’s if you were standing there by yourself. But being a part of the crowd carries you away from your personal identity, and you feel free to do things you normally would not do.
When someone is in a non-crowd situation their self awareness acts as a regulator of their behaviour. However, in a crowd situation, the focus is outward, as there is lots of stimulation to attend to. This shifts the focus outwards and their self-awareness declines. This means that the regulator of behaviour is weakened and uncharacteristic behaviour is likely to occur.

The two types of self-awareness (within context):

Not only does the individual have the stimulation from the crowd around them, but also, more often than not, they are focusing on a game and so self-awareness is diminished. This makes uncharacteristic behaviour such as aggression even more likely.

There are two types of self-awareness put forward by Prentice-Dunn & Rogers. (1982):
(1) Public self-awareness.
(2) Private self-awareness.

Public self-awareness is concerned with the appraisal of other people whereas private self-awareness is concerned with the awareness of one’s own thoughts and feelings.

Both of these can be affected by a crowd. In the case of public awareness the number of people apprasing an individual is multiplied. There is also a feeling of diffusion of responsibility, so the focus is less on being responsible for one’s own behaviour and a feeling that responsibility is shared by those around you. In general, the inhibitions that would normally moderate your behaviour lose their effect.

With private awareness the decline in self-awareness occurs because the individual is simply distracted by the crowd. They ‘lose’ themselves. There are many environmental cues to focus upon and an individual priorities those rather than focusing on themselves.

Ø Exam hint A03: When you answer longer essay questions you need to include significant AO3 commentary on theories. One good way to achieve this is to discuss the application of the theory to practical solutions. So for each theory, work out what you can say about it in terms of application.

AO3

Gergen 1973: Deindividuated persons in dark areas became more affectionate. Therefore de-individuation need not always lead to aggression
Postmes & Spears (1988): Deindividuated people are not necessarily aggressive - Crowds may be happy and good natured – as at pop festivals
Research support for anonymity – Zimbardo found that when asking females to elicit electric shocks to each other, more severe shocks were given in the deindividuated condition (participants wore hoods that hid their faces) than the controlled condition (participants were introduced to each other and wore nametags) – There is supports for anonymity as a factor of de-individuation, causing aggression.
Deiner Et Al (1976) Studied 1300 American children “trick or treating” on Halloween. Children in disguised or in a large group behaved worse. Supports deindividuation theory.
Mullen (1986) studied lynch mobs. The greater the number of people tended to correlate with the level of violence.

Emergent Norm Theory & convergence theory:
These ideas can be used as criticisms of de-individuation. They suggest that groups or sub-cultures come together because they have some sort of similarity, (convergence) then establish their own norms (emergent norms). Often one person, or a few people will behave in a certain way which others like - so they copy. This argues against de-individuation and the faceless crowd, it does not imply aggression will result. A very good example would be the hippy culture of the 1960s

18
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Explain the role of Self-awareness in aggression and subsequent Research.

A

The of self-awareness in aggression subsequent research.

Self-awareness and Research and evaluation.

The suggestion is that there is a loss of self-awareness when in a crowd; because there is a lot to pay attention to, the focus on attention is outward and so self-awareness declines.

Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1982) describe two types of self-awareness:

public, involving the appraisal of your behaviour by other people

private, where one is aware of one’s own feelings and thoughts.

The causes of deindividuation:

Some deindividuated situations can reduceaccountability, because people who are hidden within a group cannot be easily traced or blamed for their actions. Thus, the effects of deindividuation are sometimes viewed as socially undesirable (e.g.,rioting).

However, research has shown that deindividuation also strengthensadherenceto group norms. Sometimes those norms conflict with the norms of society at large, but they are not always negative. Indeed, the effects of deindividuation can be rather inconsequential (e.g., “letting loose” on the dance floor) or even positive (e.g., helping people).

Modern theories have applied and extended early principles to understand people’s behaviour in smaller groups and in othercontexts, such as when people have the opportunity to interact with others while concealing their identity and remaining anonymous.

Following Le Bon, Festinger and his colleagues proposed that being deindividuated (in particular within a group) reduces normal constraints on behaviour and encourages people to do things they normally would not do, because they are not directly accountable for their actions. They are in a sense liberated to do what they like.

Festinger found support for that idea by demonstrating that participants who engaged in a group discussion about their parents while being dressed alike in a dimly lit room were more likely to make negative comments than were participants in acontrol group. In other words, the deindividuated situation allowed participants to express views that they would normally keep to themselves.

The effect that being in an crowd has on self-awareness:

The more the person becomes involved in the group, the less self-awareness they have, which includes knowing their morals, characters, and beliefs. These qualities start to be replaced by the identity of the group. The individual then begins to take on the morals and character that is held by the group as a whole. Think of being surrounded by fans at your favourite team’s home game.

Everybody’s yelling, some people are thumping their chests, and you’re jumping up and down. Most likely, you wouldn’t exhibit these behaviour’s if you were standing there by yourself. But being a part of the crowd carries you away from your personal identity, and you feel free to do things you normally would not do.
When someone is in a non-crowd situation their self awareness acts as a regulator of their behaviour. However, in a crowd situation, the focus is outward, as there is lots of stimulation to attend to. This shifts the focus outwards and their self-awareness declines. This means that the regulator of behaviour is weakened and uncharacteristic behaviour is likely to occur.

The two types of self-awareness (within context):

Not only does the individual have the stimulation from the crowd around them, but also, more often than not, they are focusing on a game and so self-awareness is diminished. This makes uncharacteristic behaviour such as aggression even more likely.

There are two types of self-awareness put forward by Prentice-Dunn & Rogers. (1982):
(1) Public self-awareness.
(2) Private self-awareness.

Public self-awareness is concerned with the appraisal of other people whereas private self-awareness is concerned with the awareness of one’s own thoughts and feelings.

Both of these can be affected by a crowd. In the case of public awareness the number of people apprasing an individual is multiplied. There is also a feeling of diffusion of responsibility, so the focus is less on being responsible for one’s own behaviour and a feeling that responsibility is shared by those around you. In general, the inhibitions that would normally moderate your behaviour lose their effect.

With private awareness the decline in self-awareness occurs because the individual is simply distracted by the crowd. They ‘lose’ themselves. There are many environmental cues to focus upon and an individual priorities those rather than focusing on themselves.

  • The result of this lack of self-awareness is deindividuation, which in turn leads to de-individuated behaviour, behaviour that is affected by the crowd rather than internal standards. De-individuation can occur when the identity of the individual is hidden in some way. – This type of de-individuation is less likely to lead to an aggressive behaviour as there is still self-awareness and no distraction from being part of a crowd. However, research has shown that when someone’s identity is hidden, their behaviour becomes less moral and they are more likely to cheat and act more selfishly.(Zhong et al,. 2010). This illustrates that identity is key to mediating behaviour.

Research and evaluation:

Ameta-analysiscarried out by Postmes and Spears (1998) has led to the general conclusion that the evidence for deindividuation is weak. Football crowd behaviour has been described by Ingham (1978) as being due to the group norm rituals rather than deindividuation.

The idiographic approach (Unit 7 Topic 8) would suggest that different people respond differently to the various factors implicated in crowd behaviour and so general rules cannot be applied to all people in all situation.

19
Q

AG. T3: Social explanations for aggression: Summarise the overall points for this explanation.

A

Frustration is more likely to result in aggression when nearing one’s goal in a task or when being thwarted. This is especially true if the reason for being stopped from reaching the goal seems unreasonable.

If the reason for the frustration is understandable then a person is more likely to be able to control the aggression. Aggression can come from other sources such as disinhibition when being part of a crowd.

A person is capable of reasoning about the circumstance which is giving rise to the aggression. They can then work out the most appropriate way to behave to achieve the desired goal.

For a whole community to show no aggression, such as the Amish, then the cultural beliefs must be overriding the environmental pressures to respond with aggression. If aggression was innate this should not happen. Therefore the non-aggressive behaviour must be being learnt from other members of the community.

Self-awareness usually helps a person to monitor their behaviour and act in accordance with their own moral standards, which have been acquired due to the society within which they are living. If self-awareness is lost then the person will not be able to control their feelings and will act in the same fashion as the crowd around them are acting. This is deindividuation.

Summary for Social explanations for human aggression:

The frustration–aggressionhypothesissuggests that aggression results from feelings of frustration and that aggressive behaviour is a way of relieving unpleasant feelings. However, not all types of frustration result in aggressive behaviour, not all people respond to frustration aggressively, and not all aggression is prompted by frustration. Many of the studies have low predictive validity because they rely on hypothetical situations.

Social learning theory asserts that aggression is learnt from role models in the same way as other behaviours, for example in the subject’s environment and on media like television, and there is considerable research evidence to support this. In particular, social learning theory appears to be useful in explaining why aggression is displayed in particular circumstances. If aggression were solely biological in origin, it would not be so situation-specific.

Deindividuation is when people lose their sense of individual identity, for example in a crowd. In this situation some people may follow group norms rather than individual norms and so may behave atypically, for example with aggression. However, deindividuation can also lead to pro-social behaviour, for example in a religious gathering, and is not experienced by all individuals.

20
Q

AG. T4: Institutional aggression explanations: Outline the differences between the dispositional and situational explanations.

A

Institutional Theories of Aggression.

The situational approach: prisons make people aggressive – it’s the situation to blame. Situation model = Deprivation model.

The dispositional approach: prisoners are aggressive people who make the prison violent. = Importation model.

21
Q

AG. T4: Institutional aggression explanations: Outline the situational approach for aggression and the Deprivation model, and the dysfunctional institution explanation.

A

The Situational Approach: Sykes’ (1958) Deprivation Model

AO1

• Some institutions have harsh living conditions, such as prisons, army camps, refugee camp This is less of a problem if the deprivation is for a good reason; if you were on a “round the world yacht race” or a mountaineering trip you have positive attitudes to keep you going.
• Some institutions, deprive people of things they want, which reinforces the feelings of rejection from society, causing them to become more aggressive. E.g.

liberty,

autonomy,

goods and services,

sexual relationships,

communication with loved ones

security

Sykes (1958) suggested that deprivation within prisons could prompt aggression. This is where key needs are not being satisfied, such as communication with loved ones.

Five types of deprivation have been identified to have an effect on behaviour – deprivation of:

Liberty.

Autonomy.

Goods and services.

Sexual relationships.

Security.

Mnemonic prompt:
(L)ouise(A)nd(G)ary(H)ave(S)miles.

Evidence that a lack of autonomy leads to violence comes from the work of Blomberg and Lucken (2000), who showed that the frustration of having to seek permission to carry out basic acts such as washing leads to feelings of anger. Johnston (1991) and Lahm (2008) have shown that overcrowding plays a part in increasing aggressive behaviour. Overcrowding leads to competition for resources and reflects the evolutionary idea of aggression serving as a survival instinct.
Cheeseman (2003) identified frustration, leading to aggression, as being created by boredom due to a lack of stimulation.

The five Types of Deprivation in depth (as outlined in textbook):

The Deprivation model sees the loss of key needs, such as frequent communication with loved ones outside of prison, ad being the root to aggressive behaviour.

• This deprivation causes stress and frustration which leads to an aggressive sub-culture. But this only applies to places with harsh conditions: E.g. in prison, army, refugee camp etc. Less likely to be a problem if the deprivation is for a good reason; Eg. fitness & diet camp.
Effects

The general environment becomes dangerous and aggressive.

Some people retreat, back down, hide in their cells.

Others compete in order to get what they want.

Getting a tough reputation is very important in order to get respect and not be a victim.

AO3

Support for Situational Model
McCorkle (1995) In a study of 317 United States prisons, poor facilities and overcrowding were found to influence levels of violence.
Franklin (2006): Age and overcrowding led to aggression, with younger inmates (18-30) being most aggressive in conditions of overcrowding. Her Majesty’s Prison Woodhill: Major improvements at this prison included less noise, better ventilation, attractive views and especially less crowding. This led to a massive improvement in behavior in the 1990s.
Criticism
Harer and Steffensmeir (1996) found that age, race and criminal background were the only variables which affected levels of aggression. This strongly argues for the importational model, not the deprivation model.

The Situational Approach: Dysfunctional Institutionsl

AO1

Another situational argument is that the prisons themselves are dysfunctional. Milgram believed that people are loyal to the hierarchy of the organisation, but sometime the hierarchy encourages cruel behavior.
Much of Milgram’s thinking was influenced by events of the holocaust in Germany. Here the institutional aggression was on the part of the guards, rather than the prisoners.
The Stanford Prison Experiment
Zimbardo found that ordinary students became aggressive and cruel when they took on the role of being a prison guard. At the time of the Zimbardo experiment there were many prisons in the united states where conditions were extremely poor, violent and overcrowded. Some even used the prisoners as slave labour on prison farms. Zimbardo’s experiment strongly supports the situational approach.
Historical Context
At the time of theZimbardo experimentthere were many prisons in the united states where conditions were extremely poor, violent and overcrowded. Some even used the prisoners as slave labour on prison farms. Zimbardo’s experiment strongly supports the situational approach.
Features of dysfunctional Power Systems (Zimbardo)

Isolated from the outside world

Own set of values

Cohesive group; guards don’t question orders

Under pressure to act quickly

Difficult situation to manage

Out-group seen as troublemakers

22
Q

AG. T4: Institutional aggression explanations: Outline the distopstional approach for aggression and the Importation model.

A

Dispositional Explanation: The Importation Model

The Dispositional involves the characteristics that offenders bring into the prison with them. Situational is to do with the circumstances of the prison itself.

AO1

Dispositional explanation:

John Irwin and Donald Cressey (1962) put forward the importation model: this postulates that inmates bring into prison their world of sub-cultures of criminality. They possess characteristics that tend to emphasise aggressive behaviour; these can come from biochemical imbalances or inherited genetic factors. They may also have learnt their aggression from their environment, as you saw with SLT. Therefore these prisoners are acting no differently from how they would in their home environment. Addiction to alcohol and drugs can be a serious issue with offenders and it is also important to consider their behaviour in their home environments.

Irwin and Cressey identified three prison sub-cultures:

  1. criminal/thief sub-culture.
  2. convict sub-culture.
  3. conventional or ‘straight’ sub-culture.

The Importation Model focuses on the characteristics of the individual.
Irwin and Cressey argue that the prisoners import their aggressive tendencies into prison with them and this is why the rate of violence is high.
A prison is a violent place because aggressive people are in there. Their aggressive attitudes become part of its nature. It’s a dispositional approach because everything depends on the attitudes of the prisoners. This may also apply to other groups and institutions; The army / Extreme political groups / Street gangs.
Irwin and Cressy 1962: People who are sent to prison already have well established criminal behavior patterns. Prisoners were often gang members before going to prison and their loyalties and relationships are continued in the prison environment.
They also have certain learned patterns of behavior – “The code of the Streets”. They may also have problems which cause problems with relationships. E.g, Lack of self-control - Delisi (2011); Impulsive, anti-social - Wang & Diamond (2003).

AO3

Support for Irwin and Cressy / importation model
Men who were members of gangs before they went to prison are more likely to be involved in violent offences whilst in prison. Drury and Delisi (2011)
Mears (2013) believed that the code of the street is imported into prison and is the fundamental cause of aggression.
Poole and Regoli 1983: Violence before prison was the best indicator of violence inside prison. This supports the importation model.
Fischer (2001) Segregating gang members inside prison, so that they did not come into conflicts with other gangs, led to a 50% reduction in assaults.
Criticism of Importation Model
Delisi (2004) found that gang members were NOT more violent than other prisoners. However, this is a rather weak piece of research as it does not allow for the fact that those gang members had already been segregated away from other gang members. The importation model does not really explain why some organisations act aggressively when they are made up of good people supposed to act sensibly. Police officers, school teachers, traffic wardens, psychiatric nurses, and salesmen are all members of organisations which have sometimes been accused of acting in an aggressive way and yet these are very law abiding people who joined those organisations willingly and for good reasons.

Exam Tip:

In January 2012 there was a short question (4 marks) which just said; Describe one experiment which investigated Institutional Aggression. A short summary of Zimbardo was all that was needed.

23
Q

AG. T4: Institutional aggression explanations: Outline the benefit of an interactionist approach to prison violence between Dispositional/ Situational factors.

A

Jane’s notes on the discussion (in reply to my notes):

As so often in this course you need to focus on the question set. If it asks you about a specific model to evaluate you bring into your discussion some evaluation, show the examiner you have studied the course materials and are aware of the other models. It really depends on the question set and if evaluation is required from different perspectives or models. If it is asking you to describe a model that you should focus on this. All models have pros and cons but you should use the evidence provided to argue the case.

So using evidence from the course study units and book you should evaluate and back up your discussions with researchers and research. This area is well discussed in the materials.

When answering a question, check the focus of it and then draw all the materials you have studied together and argue from different perspectives if this is required to answer it.

The situational model and the dispositional model both provide an explanation for some of the reasons why aggression occurs in a much higher rate in institutionalised settings, such as the prison environment, compared to everyday settings.

The difference between the two is that the situational model proposes that increased aggression levels occur as a result of factors within an institutionalised setting, while the dispositional model focuses on individual characteristics as an explanation of aggression, and is therefore more idiographic in nature than the situational explanation.

The impact of these two suggestions means that various factors are considered when managing aggression within an institution. For example, using programmes such as anger management targeted at individual experiences and dispositions with regards to the dispositional model. The situational model also provides a basis for practical methods of reducing aggression such as creating a more comfortable environment for inmates as well as adopting a more sympathetic approach towards them.

Perhaps a problem with this work and the Importation model being more Idiographic, rather than nomothetic is that, while it takes into account the individual’s experiences and dispositions, it does not clarify the different extents that different institutional settings have on aggression in relation to this.

The case for interaction.

Both the importation and the situational model play a role in aggression in prisons and it seems likely that it is the interaction between the two that is important.

The importation model appears to better explain aggression towards other inmates, whilst the situational model is better at explaining aggression towards prison staff.

· Both models probably contribute, depending on the individual act.

Key points:

A prisoner might enter a prison with an aggressive personality, which may be caused by high testosterone levels. Alternatively, they may have learnt to be aggressive from role models within their community.

The three sub-cultures which have been identified are the criminal/thief sub-culture, which will encompass hardened criminals and repeat offenders; the convict sub-culture, which often includes people from deprived areas who are familiar with gang culture; the conventional sub-culture, people usually new to prison and probably one-time-only offenders.

An idiographic approach refers to treating people as individuals. The dispositional approach to explaining aggression in prisons looks at violence in prison by considering the individual mix of factors a prisoner brings with them into prison. Each person will respond differently to the prison regime depending on his or her own unique set of factors.

Pre-institutional violence does not explain aggression in adult prisons very well. This shows that the dispositional model is not useful when trying to explain violence in adult prisons, but it can explain it in juvenile institutions. Therefore the model cannot be generalised to all institutions.

Organisational factors include the rules and regulations, such as when inmates are expected to go to bed – inmates might object to being told when they have to retire to their cells at night. Physical factors include the level of overcrowding – cramped conditions can lead to considerable stress. Staff characteristics might include prejudiced attitudes to certain ethnic groups, which might conflict with particular inmates’ opinions.

The five areas of deprivation identified by Sykes are liberty, autonomy, goods and services, heterosexual relationships and security.

A prison could reduce aggression levels by increasing security with use of CCTV cameras and have sleeping arrangements organised so that violent inmates do not share a cell with a vulnerable person.

The effect on an inmate of situations within a prison will vary depending on the individual characteristic of that inmate. Also the situationalvariableswill vary depending on the type of prisoners within the prison. One cannot be considered without the other.

Summary for Institutional aggression:

There are two main explanations for aggression in prisons.

The dispositional explanation is concerned with the characteristics that individuals import into the prison (importation model). This is an idiographic approach and one that best explains violence against fellow inmates. This model works better for young offenders’ institutions than for adult prisons.

The situational explanation is concerned with factors within the prison setting itself. This is a nomothetic approach and this model best accounts for aggression against prison staff.

The three types of situational factors implicated ininstitutional aggressionare organisational, physical and staff characteristics.

The deprivation model of how prison may encourage aggression in offenders relates to organisational and physical indicators. Prisoners are deprived of:

liberty,

autonomy,

goods and services,

heterosexual relationships

and security.

(L)ouise(A)nd(G)ary(H)ave(S)miles.
Different research studies prioritise different deprivations.

Institutional aggressionis arguably best explained by an interactionist approach based on both dispositional and situational factors.

24
Q

AG: T5. Media influences on aggression: Describe the influences of the media on aggression, including the effects of computer games.

A

Computer Games

In recent years computer games have replaced film as the target of claims that children are taking on immoral attitudes and copying violence. Especially those involving violence, especially first person “shoot-em-ups” “Grand Theft Auto” is a very good example.

AO1

• Five psychological theories could be mentioned to support the view that repeated exposure to video game violence may lead to real life aggression:
• 1. Learning theory [Skinner]
Everything you have ever learned about Operant Conditioning can be beautifully applied to this argument. The computer game is the world’s most effective “Skinner Box”.
The human is conditioned to think in patterns which have been pre-programmed into the machine. Basic ideas are taught in the basic levels and behavior is constantly shaped to conform to the rules of the game. Every act, every single click on the mouse, is instantly rewarded, by the computer’s response. Mistakes are instantly punished.
• 2. Learning theory [Bandura]
Attention  retention  production  motivation
Individuals model the aggressive acts in the game. Some characters, and some types of behavior, are more likely to be copied because they are seen as attractive and appropriate etc. There is no sense of real punishment for making mistakes – just game over and start again. This creates disinhibition, individuals unconsciously feel that if they commit aggression they will not be punished.
• 3. Social Cognitive Observational Learning Theory [an updated version of Bandura]
Psychologists have identified certain mechanisms which explain why we learn and copy behavior:

Schemas: Models which help us understand the world [Grebner 1994]

Normative beliefs: social rules and explanations [Guerra Et. Al.]

Cognitive Priming: What connects to what in the brain [Berkowitz, Huesmann]

Cognitive Scripts: A pattern of behavior we have ready to deal with certain situations

So the films don’t suddenly turn a person violent, but they might slowly cause the development of anti-social attitudes. This could be more effective in certain types of people [not very intelligent, have no positive role model, feel hard done by in life].
• 4. The General Aggression Model [Anderson and Dill]
This model brings together elements of Social learning and Cognitive Priming Theory and suggests that if we live in a violent environment – such as a war zone, we will adapt to it, our thoughts, feelings and actions will be based around violence and that is how we will survive. But could over-exposure to gaming have the same effects?
Evidence for General Aggression Model: Meta-Analysis Findings: Anderson et al. [2004] 35 studies examined Found that video game violence exposure is related to: increases in aggressive affect, cognition and behavior increases in physiological arousal; decreases in helping behavior.
• 5. Neurological Effects
Ritterfield and Mathiak [2006] – Participants were subjected to a functional magnetic resonance imaging scan whilst playing a violent video game. It appeared to suggest that emotional areas of the cortex are to some extent “switched off” during the game, perhaps an adaptive mechanism which permits an animal to focus on survival. This is the same as happens when engaged in real acts of violence.

AO3

Strength – RWA – We can show more non-aggressive models and promote more prosocial behavior that is rewarded in the media. Aggressive behavior should always be shown to be punished – Research can be used to reduce the effect of computer games on aggression.
Weakness – Measures of aggression are artificial – Experimental studies compare participants who play a violent computer game with those who play a non-violent computer game but it is difficult to be certain that the two are equivalent in terms of game dimensions and the complexity of keys used – Casts a doubt on the validity of experimental studies and the link between media violence and aggression.
Weakness -Confounding variablesin longitudinal studies – Over lengthly periods of time, many sources of aggression interact with media influences such a role models therefore it is difficult to separate them and assess contributions to aggressive behavior – It is impossible to conclude that violent media rather than confounding variables have affected aggression.
Weakness – Publication bias – There is a tendency for only statistically significant findings to be published which is a particular problem for meta-analyses because they generally only include published studies – Creates a false impression that the effects of violent media on aggression are greater than they actually are.

25
Q

AG: T5. Media influences on aggression: Describe the influences of the media on aggression, including the role of cognitive priming.

A

Cognitive Priming

AO1

• Cognitive priming is based on the idea that memory works through association. It therefore contends that events and media images can stimulate related thoughts in the minds of audience members. For example, if we have often seen clowns throwing custard pies at one another, then when we encounter a custard pie in real life we may think about throwing it at someone.
• A schema is a model of what we think normally happens. We assume that our parents will feed us and our friends will be pleased to see us because that is what normally happens.
• A cognitive script is a way of dealing with a situation. We have learned that in a hotel restaurant we sit down and wait to be served, but in a burger bar we line up at the counter.
• Berkowitz thinks watching violent movies could lead to storing schemas and cognitive scripts which involve aggression EG. the students in the Stanford Prison experiment had never been in a real prison but they may have had a schema based on movies they had seen. EG. Students who play “Grand Theft Auto” might develop a cognitive script for what to do when traffic lights turn amber. This may be different from the way their Grandma drives!
• Priming means that a particular event, or an image or even a word may be associated with these thoughts. We call that a trigger. When we encounter the trigger we may respond in the way we have been primed. EG. a football comes bouncing towards me - without thinking I put out my foot to stop it or kick it back, but if it’s a cricket ball I would pick it up and throw it back. I am primed to respond differently to the cricket ball. So Berkowitz argues that we learn anti-social attitudes from the media and these are associated with certain triggers.

AO3

Steve Berkowitz [1984] did an experiment involving an argument in an office. In condition A there was a baseball bat on the side of the desk. In condition B there was a badminton racquet. Berkowitz found the presence of the baseball bat led to more aggressive responses.
Bushman [1998] Participants who had watched a violent film responded more quickly to aggressive words than those who had watched a non-violent film.
Anderson and Dill [2000] Found that playing a violent computer game led to more aggressive thoughts. They claimed that even playing the game just once could be having this effect, although the effect might only be short term.
Zelli [1995] found that cognitive priming could be used to make people suspicious of the intentions of others. This in turn led the people who had been primed to act in a more aggressive manner.
Murray [2007] – used fMRI scans to study children’s brains when watching violent and non-violent TV programmes. Violent films led to increased activity in those areas which deal with emotion, arousal and attention – not surprising – but also in the areas used to storeepisodic memory. This supports the suggestion that children can store scripts.
Atkin [2003] found that priming was more pronounced when the media was more realistic. However this may not simply mean it “looked better” it might relate to how much the participant believed it was realistic.

26
Q

AG: T5. Media influences on aggression: Briefly Outline the role that desensitisation plays in media influences.

A

Desensitisation

AO1

• Media violence leads to aggressive behavior by removing the anxiety about violence. Someone who becomes desensitized to violence may therefore perceive it as more ‘normal’ and be more likely to engage in violence themselves. Effects may involve a reduction in physiological arousal (e.g. heart rate) when exposed to real violence or psychological response (e.g. having less sympathy for a victim).
• In the natural world a certain level of natural fear should make people hold back from violent situations. The desensitisation argument suggests that if children watch too much violence on TV they will be less scared and therefore more open to aggressive activity.
• People become less likely to notice violence, or in real life. They have less sympathy for victims of violence. They have less negative attitudes towards violence. [Mullin and Linz 1995]
Measuring desensitisation
• Desensitisation can be monitored by physical indicators of stress, such as heart beat and galvanic skin response. [Linz 1989]
• Carnagey [2007] found that experienced computer gamers show less of a reaction to a film of real life violence.
Effects of Desensitisation
• Bushman and Anderson [2009] found that desensitisation made people less likely to help others in unpleasant situations.
• Dolf Zillman suggested that if we survive real life danger we feel good afterwards [winners] During an action movie we feel excited and stimulated. Later we want that excitement again but we become de-sensitised so we need more scary films to get us excited. This could transfer to seeking violence in real life.

27
Q

AG: T5. Media influences on aggression: Secondly briefly Outline the role that desensitisation plays in media influences.

A

Disinhibition

AO1

• Disinhibition explains that watching or playing violent media may change the standards of what is considered acceptable behavior. The media gives aggressive behavior social approval, especially where effects on victims are minimized and appear justified.
• Normally we act in certain ways because we have been socialised to know what is right and wrong. We get aroused and excited by a film or a game and this causes us to lose our inhibitions, acting in a more extreme manner till the excitement dies down.
• Long term Disinhibition: Too much violent TV can change our actual moral values so that we see more violent standards of behavior as acceptable. One aspect of this is that we often see acts of violence going unpunished in movies or games and this could lead to disinhibition.

AO3

Individual factors [Collins 1989] make disinhibition more or less likely:

Violent home background

Physical punishment of children

Younger viewers

Children with low intelligence

Children who believe their heroes are realistic

Children who believe the media reflects real life

• Disinhibition less likely if Strong family norms against violence or where adults discuss issues from the film with their children.
• Research support for disinhibition – Berkowitz found that participants who saw a film depicting aggression as vengeance gave more fake electric shocks to a confederate – The media disinhibits aggression by presenting it as justified and removing social constraints.
• Disinhibition can explain the effect of cartoon violence – Children learn social norms through cartoon characters as the aggression they carry out is socially normative, especially when it goes unpunished – Children learn that aggression is rewarding and achieves goals in a social acceptable way therefore are more prone to copy it.

There are several explanations for this. Suler, 2004:

anonymity and invisibility.

solipsistic introjection.

minimisation of authority.

The effects stated above described in depth:

  1. Anonymity and invisibility – of the person is assured while playing a computer game. This means that responsibility for behaviour is perceived as reduced. This is a similar effect to de-individuation when an individual cannot be recognised because they are in the dark or their identity is obscured in some way. (See page 565 in textbook).
  2. Solipsistic introjection – which is the feeling of becoming cognitively merged with the actor in the game. This occurs when an alter is selected to act within the virtual world and by playing as that alter the game becomes part of the alter. Therefore uncharacteristic behaviour may occur because the individual not acting as himself/herself.
  3. Minimisation of authority – occurs in computer games as there is often no law enforcement or awareness of potential legal consequences as there might be in everyday life. This means that behaviour which would be classified as criminal is acted out as it is a virtual world.
28
Q

AG: T5. Media influences on aggression: Key overall points.

A

Hypothetical scenarios suffer from predictive validity issues. Just because someone says that they would act in a particular way when asked, this does notmeanthat they would act in this way in the real situation.

Matthewset al.(2006) found decreased activity in the prefrontal lobe of the brain when playing violent computer games. This area of the brain is involved in control of emotions.

Kestenbaum and Weinstein (1985) found that playing computer games allowed adolescent males to release their aggression and feel calmer.

Desensitisation allows a film to appear less scary on a second viewing.

Hiltzet al.(1989) used pen names with their participants, rather than actual names, in a computer conference and found that there was more pro-social behaviour than when actual names were used. However the effect was not statistically significant.

Solipsistic introjection is when a person experiences a feeling of becoming merged with a character within a game. The player then does not behave as themselves but instead as the game’s character.

Introverts are less likely than extroverts to be distracted by outside events and so can become more immersed within the game environment, possibly leading to a greater level of disinhibition.

A correlational relationship does not allow for a decision as to which factor is the cause and which is the effect. In cognitive priming it could be that aggressive and violent people choose to watch more violent TV, not that violent TV causes people to be aggressive.

Summary for Media influences on aggression:

Unit 10 as a whole has introduced you to a widerangeof theories to explain aggressive behaviour, from the biological to the social, and how these interact with each other in their effect. You have seen that what would have been a biological advantage in the evolution of the human species might nowadays be a problem within society.

From this background of theory, you moved on to look at a specific example of aggressive behaviour, i.e. that of prison life. Topic 4 explored the various reasons why prisons should be a special case of aggressive behaviour. In particular, you were encouraged to tackle this topic in terms of solutions to manage thisinstitutional aggression. In Topic 5 you have examined the possible roles for desensitisation, disinhibition and cognitive priming in behavioural responses to violence displayed in the media. You have considered whether there is a problem in our society emanating from exposure to media violence, with particular reference to the playing of computer games.