Aggression- A03 Flashcards
neural and hormonal mechanisms
The amygaldas role in aggression is backed up by research
- Pardini carried out a longitudinal study of 56 male pps
- MRI scans showed that those with lower amygalda volumes had higher levels of aggression & violence
- This suggests that the lower amygalda plays a key role in evaluating the importance of sensory information and that lower amygalda volume compromises this ability,, making a violent response more likely
neural and hormonal mechanisms
The role of the hippocampus in aggression is supported by MRI scans of hippocampus asymmetry
- Reine et al studied 2 groups of violent criminals.
- ones that faced conviction (unsuccessful psychopaths)
- others who had evaded the law (successful psychopaths)
- The latter group were considered to be cold, calculating criminals
- The former acted more impulsively
- MRI scans revealed asymmetries in the hippocampus (part of limbic system) in the ‘unsuccessful group’ an imbalance presumed to have arisen early in brain development
- The researchers suggested this asymmetry might impair the ability of the hippocampus and the amygalda to work together so that emotional information is not processed correctly leading to inappropriate verbal & physical responses
neural and hormonal mechanisms
The issue of cause & effect affects out understanding of the difference between neural & hormonal difference & aggression.
- most research into neural & hormonal influences on aggression is correlational due to ethical reasons (opportunities to experimentally manipulate brain structures & hormones are limited)
- When two variables are correlated it is impossible to establish which one is the cause of the other, or if a 3rd variable is involved
- this risks oversimplifying other factors e.g serotonin & aggression as other factors which may influence aggression are overlooked
- there is no alternative without tasing ethical issues. Many researchers consider research w/ non human animals ethically acceptable e.g castration of male rats but this raises generalisability
neural and hormonal mechanisms
There is some support for the serotonin deficiency hypothesis to explain human & non human aggression
- Rosado compared a sample of 80 dogs of various breeds that had been referred to Spanish veterinary hospitals for their aggressive behaviour
- Control sample of 19 dogs that didn’t show aggressive behaviour
- Aggressive dogs averaged 278 units of serotonin
- Non aggressive averaged 387 units
- Duke et al: meta-analysis of 175 studies found an inverse relationship between serotonin levels & aggression i.e lower serotonin, higher the aggression
- suggests serotonin could be a casual factor in aggression
neural and hormonal mechanisms
However the role of testosterone in aggression has been challenged by studies failing to show a relationship between it & human aggression
- e.g some research has shown no correlation between testosterone & actual violence in male prison inmates
- the ones that have found a relationship often involve a small prison sample of men, using self report or rating of aggression based on their crime
- Mazur suggests testosterone may actually promote status-where aggression is just one type. Some seek status through the use of aggression whilst others through wealth
- suggests the relationship between testosterone & aggression may be more complex than once thought
Genetic factors in aggression
It is also possible that the MAOA gene can explain gender differences in aggression
- offers explanation for uneven rates of violence between males & females
- Niehoff suggests that this may be a consequence of the different vulnerabilities
- MAOA gene : linked to X chromosome
- Women have 2 X chromosomes- men only have 1
- Men more likely to be affected ( e.g when they inherit an X linked gene from their parent ) wheras women second X chromosome may over ride it & expression of the abnormal MAOA gene is prevented
- women unaffected as they have 2 X chromosomes with a ‘normal gene’
Genetic factors in aggression
The role of the MAOA gene in serious violent behaviour is supported by evidence
- Tiihonen found that the MAOA-L in combination with another gene was only found in extremely violent prisoners & not offenders
- This gene combination may make it difficult for a person to control their violent urges & therefore show that the MAOA gene does play a role in violence but that other genes as well as environmental factors are also implicated
Genetic factors in aggression
However one issue with this area of research is that most studies have focused on people convicted of violent crime
- most violence occurs in people who are never convicted & those who are studied only represent a small minority
- this means the sample that findings of many studies are based on are potentially biased & certainly limited
- it’s therefore hard to draw valid conclusions about the role of genetic facts (heritability) in aggression
Genetic factors in aggression
Not only is there a problem sampling but there is also a problem of assessing aggression
- In Miles & Careys meta- analysis of 24 studies there was a greater genetic link in studies using a self report than in studies using observation which found a stronger environmental link
- this suggests different research methods yield different results. This therefore means it is difficult to accurately assess the contribution of genetic & environmental factors on aggression
Genetic factors in aggression
Thus it is difficult to establish the role of genetic factors on aggression with any certainty
-further challenged by the fact that concordance rates in MZ twins are never 100%, in fact they are less than 50% suggesting that complex behaviour is always a combination if genes and environment as shown in Caspi’s study where anti-social behaviour only occurred if those MAOA-L males had been maltreated as children (explain Caspi in more detail)
ethological explanations
Evidence shows just how species specific FAP’s are
- it’s known that most animals respond to pain with aggression but the way it takes differs
- e.g Ulrich showed how the rates given electric shocks will immediately attack each other. This behaviour (FAP) follows a predictable pattern that is unique to rats
- Two hares will however both Stand up on their hind legs & start boxing each other with their front paws
- This behaviour is not known in other species & so lends support to the idea of the universal characteristics of FAP
ethological explanations
Lorenz’s idea of ‘instinctive inhibition’ is not supported by evidence
- e.g Jane Goodall observed what she called the ‘four year war’ in a national park in Tasmania. Male chimps promote community by systematically slaughtering all the members of another group in a coordinated fashion
- The violence continued despite the fact that the victims were offering signals of appeasement and defencelessnes suggesting much of behaviour is real & not ritualistic
ethological explanations
Ritualised aggression is seen in humans too & has the same benefits for non humans
In non humans ritualistic aggression prevents by preventing conflicts escalating into potentially dangerous physical aggression. Anthropological evidence suggests this advantage is also evident in human cultures e.g Chagnon found the Yanomamo people of South Africa pound their chest & club fight to settle their conflict short of more violence
-suggests that even in fairly violent cultures rituals have the affect of reducing actual aggression preventing death
ethological explanations
human FAP of aggression is no longer adaptive
Ebil-Eibesfelt indentified ‘FAP’s’ such as smiling/ eyebrow flash as a sign of greeting
- however because of the environment in which humans exist changes so rapidly Ebil-Eibesfeldt suggests FAP’s e.g aggression are no more adaptive in modern times
- The flexibility of human behaviour & the ability to respond in an ever changing environment has proved more effective than FAP
- this suggests that even though non-human species respond aggressively to specific sign stimuli human behaviour is far more varied & less predictable
ethological explanations
Fixed action patterns are not just innate but can be modified by learning
- Lehrman criticised Lorens’z instinctive inhibition explanation of aggressive behaviour. He believed Lorenz had underestimated the role of environmental factors in the development of species ‘FAP’s These environmental factors were largely the result of learning & experience interacting with innate factors
- Nowadats FAP is replaced by the term ‘behaviour pattern’ to replace the fact that these can be modified by experience. Nor is behaviour ‘fixed’
- there are subtle variations between members of the same species in the prediction of aggressive behaviour
- Suggests FAP’s that Lorenz claimed are not as fixed as he thought